1,072
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The Senses Considered as One Perceptual System

, &

References

  • Adolph, K. E. (1995). Psychophysical assessment of toddlers' ability to cope with slopes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 21, 734–750.
  • Adolph, K. E., Eppler, M., Marin, L., Weiss, I., & Clearfield, M. W. (2000). Exploration in the service of prospective control. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 441–460.
  • Adolph, K. E., Marin, L. M., & Fraisse, F. F. (2001). Learning and exploration: Lessons from infants. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 213–214.
  • Anderson, J. D. (1996). The reality of illusion: An ecological approach to cognitive film theory. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press.
  • Aristotle. (1931). De anima [On the soul]. In W. D. Ross (Ed.), The works of Aristotle translated into English. Wolten-under-Edge, UK: Clarendon Press.
  • Bahrick, L. E. (1988). Intermodal learning in infancy: Learning on the basis of two kinds of intermodal relations in audible and visible events. Child Development, 59, 197–209.
  • Bingham, G. P., & Stassen, M. G. (1994). Monocular egocentric distance information generated by head movement. Ecological Psychology, 6, 219–238.
  • Chiari, L., Rocchi, L., & Cappello, A. (2002). Stabilometric parameters are affected by anthropometry and foot placement. Clinical Biomechanics, 17, 666–677.
  • Darlington, C. L., Ross, A., King, J., & Smith, P. F. (2001). Menstrual cycle effects on postural stability but not optokinetic function. Neuroscience Letters, 307, 147–150.
  • Dupuy, J.-P. (2008). The mechanization of the mind (M. B. DeBevoise, Trans.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Fajen, B. R. (2005). Calibration, information, and control strategies for braking to avoid a collision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 480–501.
  • Faugloire, E., Bardy, B. G., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2006). The dynamics of learning new postural patterns. Journal of Motor Behavior, 38, 299–312.
  • Fedo, C. M., & Whitehouse, M. J. (2002). Metasomatic origin of quartz-pyroxene rock, Akila, Greenland, and implications for Earth's earliest life. Science, 296, 1448–1452.
  • Fitzpatrick, P., Carello, C., Schmidt, R. C., & Corey, D. (1994). Haptic and visual perception of an affordance for upright posture. Ecological Psychology, 6, 265–288.
  • Flom, R. A., & Bahrick, L. E. (2001). The global array: Not new to infant researchers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 221–222.
  • Fowler, C. A. (2004). Speech as a supramodal or amodal phenomenon. In G. A. Calver, C. Spence, & B. E. Stein (Eds.), The handbook of multisensory processes. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Fowler, C. A., & Dekle, D. J. (1991). Listening with eye and hand: Cross-modal contributions to speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 816–828.
  • Gibson, E. J. (1983). Development of knowledge about intermodal unity: Two views. In L. S. Liben (Ed.), Piaget and the foundations of knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gibson, E. J., & Pick, A. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Gibson, E. J., & Walker, A. S. (1984). Development of knowledge of visual-tactual affordances of substance. Child Development, 55, 453–460.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1950). The perception of the visual world. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1986). The ecological approach to visual perception. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ( Original work published 1979)
  • Gibson, J. J., Olum, P., & Rosenblatt, F. (1955). Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. The American Journal of Psychology, 68, 372–385.
  • Heft, H. (1988). The development of Gibson's ecological approach to perception. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 8, 325–334.
  • Hochberg, J. (1971). Perception II: Space and movement. In J. Kling & L. Rigg (Eds.), Woodworth and Schlosberg's experimental psychology (pp. 474–550). New York, NY: Holt, Reinhart, & Winston.
  • Kerrigan, D. C., Todd, M. K., & Croce, U. D. (1998). Gender differences in joint biomechanics during walking: Normative study in young adults. American Journal of Physical Medicine & Rehabilitation, 77, 2–7.
  • Kim, J. W., Eom, G. M., Kim, C. S., Kim, D. H., Lee, J. H., Park, B. K., & Hong, J. (2010). Sex differences in the postural sway characteristics of young and elderly subjects during quiet natural standing. Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 10, 191–198.
  • Lai, P. P. K., Leung, A. K. L., Li, A. N. M., & Zhang, M. (2008). Three-dimensional gait analysis of obese adults. Clinical Biomechanics, 23, S2–S6.
  • Lamb, T. D. (2011). Evolution of the eye. Scientific American, 305, 64–69.
  • Lee, D. A. (1980). The optic flow field: The foundation of vision. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 290, 169–179.
  • Lee, D. N. (1990). Getting around with light or sound. In R. Warren & A. H. Wertheim (Eds.), Perception and control of self-motion (pp. 487–506). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Lombardo, T. J. (1987). The reciprocity of perceiver and environment: The evolution of James J. Gibson's ecological psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Loula, F., Prasad, S., Harber, K., & Shiffrar, M. (2005). Recognizing people from their movement. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 210–220.
  • Mace, W. M. (1977). James J. Gibson's strategy for perceiving: Ask not what's inside your head, but what your head is inside of. In R. E. Shaw & J. Bransford (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 43–65). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Mantel, B., Stoffregen, T. A., Campbell, A., & Bardy, B. G. (2015). Exploratory movement generates higher-order information that is sufficient for accurate perception of scaled egocentric distance. Plos ONE, 10(4), e0120025. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120025
  • Mark, L. S. (1987). Eyeheight-scaled information about affordances: A study of sitting and stair climbing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 361–370.
  • Mark, L. S., Balliett, J. A., Craver, K. D., Douglas, S. D., & Fox, T. (1990). What an actor must do in order to perceive the affordance for sitting. Ecological Psychology, 2, 325–366.
  • Morris, M. E., Huxham, F., McGinley, J., Dodd, K., & Iansek, R. (2001). The biomechanics and motor control of gait in Parkinson disease. Clinical Biomechanics, 16, 459–470.
  • Neisser, U. (1976). Cognition and reality. San Francisco, CA: Freeman.
  • Oman, C. M. (1982). A heuristic mathematical model for the dynamics of sensory conflict and motion sickness. Acta Otolaryngologica, 44, Supplement 392.
  • Oudejans, R. R. D., Michaels, C. F., Bakker, F. C., & Dolne, M. A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 879–891.
  • Peper, L., Bootsma, R. J., Mestre, D. R., & Bakker, F. C. (1994). Catching balls: How to get the hand to the right place at the right time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 591–612.
  • Pittenger, J. B., & Dent, C. H. (1988). A mechanism for the direct perception of change: The example of bacterial chemotaxis. Perception, 17, 119133.
  • Reed, E. S. (1988). James J. Gibson and the psychology of perception. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  • Riccio, G. E., & Stoffregen, T. A. (1990). Gravitoinertial force versus the direction of balance in the perception and control of orientation. Psychological Review, 97, 135–137.
  • Richardson, D. C., Dale, R., & Kirkham, N. Z. (2007). The art of conversation is coordination: Common ground and the coupling of eye movements during dialog. Psychological Science, 18, 407–413.
  • Richardson, M. J., Shockley, K., Fajen, B. R., Riley, M. A., & Turvey, M. T. (2008). Ecological psychology: Six principles for an embodied-embedded approach to behavior. In P. Calvo & T. Gomila (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach (pp. 161–187). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.
  • Riley, M. A., Wagman, J. B., Santana, M.-V., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (2002). Perceptual behavior: Recurrence analysis of a haptic exploratory procedure. Perception, 31, 481–510.
  • Rosenblum, L. D., Dorsi, J., & Dias, J. W. (2016). The impact and status of Carol Fowler's supramodal theory of multisensory speech perception. Ecological Psychology, 28, 262–294.
  • Rosenblum, L. D., & Saldaña, H. M. (1996). An audiovisual test of kinematic primitives for visual speech perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 318–331.
  • Sanchez, M., Allison, W. W. M., Alner, G. J., Ayres, D. S., Barrett, W. L., Border, P. M., … West, N. (2003). Observation of atmospheric neutrino oscillations in Soudan 2. Physics Review D, 68, 113004.
  • Schmit, J. M., Regis, D. I., & Riley, M. A. (2005). Dynamic patterns of postural sway in ballet dancers and track athletes. Experimental Brain Research, 163, 370–378.
  • Schmit, J. M., Riley, M. A., Dalvi, A., Sahay, A., Shear, P. K., Shockley, K. D., & Pun, R. Y. K. (2006). Deterministic center of pressure patterns characterize postural instability in Parkinson's disease. Experimental Brain Research, 168, 357–367.
  • Shaw, R., Turvey, M. T., & Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimerd & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Sherrick, C. (1967). A brave new perceptual world. Contemporary Psychology, 12, 529–530.
  • Shockley, K., Santana, M.-V., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Mutual interpersonal postural constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 326–332.
  • Slowinski, P., Zhai, C., Alderisio, F., Salesse, R., Gueugnon, M., Marin, L., … Tsaneva-Atanasova, K. (2016). Dynamic similarity promotes interpersonal coordination in joint action. Journals of the Royal Society, Interface, 13, 20151093.
  • Soechting, J. F., & Flanders, M. (1992). Moving in three-dimensional space: Frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 167–191.
  • Solomon, H. Y., & Turvey, M. T. (1988). Haptically perceiving the distances reachable with hand-held objects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14, 404–427.
  • Stevens, S. C., & Parsons, M. G. (2002). Effects of motion at sea on crew performance: A survey. Marine Technology, 39, 29–47.
  • Stoffregen, T. A. (1997). Filming the world: An essay review of Anderson's. The reality of illusion. Ecological Psychology, 9, 161–177.
  • Stoffregen, T. A. (2014, June). Exploring Len Mark's affordances. Talk presented at a North American meeting of the International Society for Ecological Psychology, Oxford OH.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., & Bardy, B. G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 195–261.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., Bardy, B. G., Smart, L. J., & Pagulayan, R. J. (2003). On the nature and evaluation of fidelity in virtual environments. In L. J. Hettinger & M. W. Haas (Eds.), Virtual and adaptive environments: Applications, implications, and human performance issues (pp. 111–128). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., Gorday, K. M., Sheng, Y. Y., & Flynn, S. B. (1999). Perceiving affordances for another person's actions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 25, 120–136.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., & Riccio, G. E. (1988). An ecological theory of orientation and the vestibular system. Psychological Review, 95, 3–14.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., & Riccio, G. E. (1991). An ecological critique of the sensory conflict theory of motion sickness. Ecological Psychology, 3, 159–194.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., Yang, C. M., & Bardy, B. G. (2005). Affordance judgments and nonlocomotor body movement. Ecological Psychology, 17, 75–104.
  • Stoffregen, T. A., Yoshida, K., Villard, S., Scibora, L., & Bardy, B. G. (2010). Stance width influences postural stability and motion sickness. Ecological Psychology, 22, 169–191.
  • Thelen, E., & Spencer, J. P. (1998). Postural control during reaching in young infants: A dynamic systems approach. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 22, 507–514.
  • Teplá, L., Procházková, M., Svoboda, Z., & Janura, M. (2014). Kinematic analysis of the gait in professional ballet dancers. Acta Gymnica, 44, 85–91.
  • Tretriluxana, J., Gordon, J., & Winstein, C. J. (2008). Manual asymmetries in grasp pre-shaping and transport–grasp coordination. Experimental Brain Research, 188, 305–315.
  • von Fieandt, K. (1967). Review. Synthese, 17, 230–232.
  • Vuillerme, N., Baptiste, A., & Rougier, P. (2007). Trunk extensor muscles fatigue affects undisturbed postural control in young healthy adults. Clinical Biomechanics, 22, 489–494.
  • Wagman, J. B., Caputo, S. E., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2016). Hierarchical nesting of affordances in a tool use task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 42, 1627–1642.
  • Walker-Andrews, A. (2001). Infants, too, are global perceivers. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 244–245.
  • Yu, Y., Bardy, B. G., & Stoffregen, T. A. (2011). Influences of head and torso movement before and during affordance perception. Journal of Motor Behavior, 43, 45–54.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.