75
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Themed Issue on Early Education and Development in Diverse Sociocultural Contexts

Cognitive Distancing Language in Parent-Child Book Sharing Among Low-Income Mexican American and Chinese American Families: Cultural Group Comparisons and Links to Children’s Executive Functions

ORCID Icon, , , &

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. MIT Press.
  • Arnold, D. H., Lonigan, C. J., Whitehurst, G. J., & Epstein, J. N. (1994). Accelerating language development through picture book reading: Replication and extension to a videotape training format. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(2), 235–243. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.86.2.235
  • Bailey, C. S., Denham, S. A., & Curby, T. W. (2013). Questioning as a component of scaffolding in predicting emotion knowledge in preschoolers. Early Child Development and Care, 183(2), 265–279. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2012.671815
  • Baker, C. E., Vernon-Feagans, L., & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2015). Fathers’ language input during shared book activities: Links to children’s kindergarten achievement. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 36, 53–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2014.11.009
  • Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x
  • Bernier, A., Carlson, S. M., Deschênes, M., & Matte-Gagné, C. (2012). Social factors in the development of early executive functioning: A closer look at the caregiving environment. Developmental Science, 15(1), 12–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01093.x
  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29(3), 180–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002
  • Blewitt, P., Rump, K. M., Shealy, S. E., & Cook, S. A. (2009). Shared book reading: When and how questions affect young children’s word learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(2), 294–304. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013844
  • Bus, A. G. (2003). Social-emotional requisites for learning to read. In A. van Kleeck, S. A. Stahl, & E. B. Bauer (Eds.), On reading books to children: Parents and teachers (pp. 3–14). Routledge.
  • Carlson, S. M. (2005). Developmentally sensitive measures of executive function in preschool children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 28(2), 595–616. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2802_3
  • Chan, M., Teng, D., Teng, Y.-P. T., & Zhou, Q. (2022). Parent emotion talk with preschoolers from low-income Mexican American and Chinese American families: Links to sociocultural factors. Social Development, 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1111/sode.12656
  • Chen, S. H., Main, A., Zhou, Q., Bunge, S. A., Lau, N., & Chu, K. (2015). Effortful control and early academic achievement of Chinese American children in immigrant families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30, 45–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.08.004
  • Chen, X., Zhang, G., Chen, H., & Li, D. (2012). Performance on delay tasks in early childhood predicted socioemotional and school adjustment nine years later: A longitudinal study in Chinese children. International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation, 1(1), 3–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026363
  • Chen, S. H., Zhou, Q., Uchikoshi, Y., & Bunge, S. A. (2014). Variations on the bilingual advantage? Links of Chinese and English proficiency to Chinese American children’s self-regulation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01069
  • Chernoff, J. J., Keuter, S., Uchikoshi, Y., Quick, H., & Manship, K. (2021). Challenges in assessing California’s diverse dual language learners. American Institute for Research. https://californiadllstudy.org/sites/default/files/2021-02/Challenges%20in%20Assessing%20California%E2%80%99s%20Diverse%20DLLs.pdf
  • Cristofaro, T. N., & Tamis LeMonda, C. S. (2012). Mother-child conversations at 36 months and at pre-kindergarten: Relations to children’s school readiness. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 12(1), 68–97. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798411416879
  • Crookes, G. (1990). The utterance, and other basic units for second language discourse analysis. Applied Linguistics, 11(2), 183–199. https://doi.org/10.1093/applin/11.2.183
  • Czwenar, I. (2014). Analysing spoken language for complexity, accuracy and fluency: Some methodological considerations. In W. Szubko-Sitarek, Ł. Salski, & P. Stalmaszczyk (Eds.), Language learning, discourse and communication: Studies in honour of Jan Majer (pp. 81–92). Springer International Publishing. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-00419-8_6
  • Dalton-Puffer, C. (2011). Content-and-language integrated learning: From practice to principles? Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 31, 182–204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0267190511000092
  • Daneri, M. P., Blair, C., Kuhn, L. J., & Investigators, F. K. (2019). Maternal language and child vocabulary mediate relations between socioeconomic status and executive function during early childhood. Child Development, 90(6), 2001–2018. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13065
  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2037–2078. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006
  • Deshmukh, R. S., Zucker, T. A., Tambyraja, S. R., Pentimonti, J. M., Bowles, R. P., & Justice, L. M. (2019). Teachers’ use of questions during shared book reading: Relations to child responses. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 49, 59–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2019.05.006
  • Dexter, C. A., & Stacks, A. M. (2014). A preliminary investigation of the relationship between parenting, parent-child shared reading practices, and child development in low-income families. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 28(3), 394–410. https://doi.org/10.1080/02568543.2014.913278
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
  • Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2019). Review of the evidence on, and fundamental questions about, efforts to improve executive functions, including working memory. In J. M. Novick, M. F. Bunting, M. R. Dougherty, & R. W. Engle (Eds.), Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience, and human development. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780199974467.003.0008
  • Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2010). Maternal discussions of mental states and behaviors: Relations to emotion situation knowledge in European American and immigrant Chinese children. Child Development, 81(5), 1490–1503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01487.x
  • Doebel, S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2016). Seeing conflict and engaging control: Experience with contrastive language benefits executive function in preschoolers. Cognition, 157, 219–226. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2016.09.010
  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. American Guidance Service.
  • Dunn, L. M., Padilla, E. R., Lugo, D. E., & Dunn, L. M. (1986). Test de vocabulario en imágenes Peabody: Adaptación Hispanoamericana. American Guidance Service.
  • Friend, M., & Bates, R. P. (2014). The union of narrative and executive function: Different but complementary. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00469
  • Fuhs, M. W., & Day, J. D. (2011). Verbal ability and executive functioning development in preschoolers at head start. Developmental Psychology, 47(2), 404–416. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021065
  • Garon, N., Bryson, S. E., & Smith, I. M. (2008). Executive function in preschoolers: A review using an integrative framework. Psychological Bulletin, 134(1), 31–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.1.31
  • Gómez, L. E., Vasilyeva, M., & Dulaney, A. (2017). Preschool teachers’ read-aloud practices in Chile as predictors of children’s vocabulary. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 52, 149–158. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2017.07.005
  • Grissom, N. M., & Reyes, T. M. (2019). Let’s call the whole thing off: Evaluating gender and sex differences in executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology Reviews, 44(1), 86–96. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41386-018-0179-5
  • Gueron-Sela, N., Camerota, M., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., Cox, M. J., & The Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2018). Maternal depressive symptoms, mother-child interactions and children’s executive function. Developmental Psychology, 54(1), 71–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000389
  • Huebner, C. E., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2005). Intervention to change parent–child reading style: A comparison of instructional methods. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 296–313. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2005.02.006
  • Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017–2036. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.010
  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Waterfall, H. R., Vevea, J. L., & Hedges, L. V. (2007). The varieties of speech to young children. Developmental Psychology, 43(5), 1062–1083. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1062
  • Kelly, R., Dissanayake, C., Ihsen, E., & Hammond, S. (2011). The relationship between symbolic play and executive function in young children. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 36(2), 21–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/183693911103600204
  • Kuchirko, Y., Tamis LeMonda, C. S., Luo, R., & Liang, E. (2016). ‘What happened next?’: Developmental changes in mothers’ questions to children. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 16(4), 498–521. https://doi.org/10.1177/1468798415598822
  • Kuhn, L. J., Willoughby, M. T., Vernon-Feagans, L., Blair, C. B., & The Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2016). The contribution of children’s time-specific and longitudinal expressive language skills on developmental trajectories of executive function. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 148, 20–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.03.008
  • Labrell, F., Deleau, M., & Juhel, J. (2000). Fathers’ and mothers’ distancing strategies towards toddlers. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24(3), 356–361. https://doi.org/10.1080/01650250050118349
  • Leung, G., & Uchikoshi, Y. (2012). Relationships among language ideologies, family language policies, and children’s language achievement: A look at Cantonese-English bilinguals in the U.S. Bilingual Research Journal, 35(3), 294–313. https://doi.org/10.1080/15235882.2012.731588
  • Llabre, M. M. (2021). Insight into the Hispanic paradox: The language hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 16(6), 1324–1336. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691620968765
  • Lu, L., & Liu, H. S. (1998). The peabody picture vocabulary test-revised in Chinese. Psychological Publishing.
  • Luo, R., Masek, L. R., Alper, R. M., & Hirsh-Pasek, K. (2022). Maternal question use and child language outcomes: The moderating role of children’s vocabulary skills and socioeconomic status. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 59, 109–120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2021.11.007
  • Luo, R., & Tamis LeMonda, C. S. (2017). Reciprocity between maternal questions and child contributions during book-sharing. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 38, 71–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2016.08.003
  • Luo, R., Tamis LeMonda, C. S., Kuchirko, Y., Ng, F., & Liang, E. (2014). Mother–child book-sharing and children’s storytelling skills in ethnically diverse, low-income families. Infant and Child Development, 23(4), 402–425. https://doi.org/10.1002/icd.1841
  • Luo, R., Tamis LeMonda, C. S., & Song, L. (2013). Chinese parents’ goals and practices in early childhood. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 843–857. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.08.001
  • Manz, P. H., Hughes, C., Barnabas, E., Bracaliello, C., & Ginsburg-Block, M. (2010). A descriptive review and meta-analysis of family-based emergent literacy interventions: To what extent is the research applicable to low-income, ethnic-minority or linguistically-diverse young children? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), 409–431. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.03.002
  • Mayer, M. (1969). Frog, where are you? Penguin Books.
  • Melzi, G., & Caspe, M. (2005). Variations in maternal narrative styles during book reading interactions. Narrative Inquiry, 15(1), 101–125. https://doi.org/10.1075/ni.15.1.06mel
  • Melzi, G., Schick, A. R., & Kennedy, J. L. (2011). Narrative elaboration and participation: Two dimensions of maternal elicitation style. Child Development, 82(4), 1282–1296. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01600.x
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Mol, S. E., Bus, A. G., De Jong, M. T., & Smeets, D. J. H. (2008). Added value of dialogic parent–child book readings: A meta-analysis. Early Education and Development, 19(1), 7–26. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409280701838603
  • Munakata, Y. (2006). Information processing approaches to development. In D. Kuhn, R. S. Siegler, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Cognition, perception, and language (6th ed., Vol. 2, pp. 426–463). John Wiley & Sons Inc.
  • Noble, C., Sala, G., Peter, M., Lingwood, J., Rowland, C., Gobet, F., & Pine, J. (2019). The impact of shared book reading on children’s language skills: A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 28, 100290. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2019.100290
  • Park, M., O’Toole, A., & Katsiaficas, C. (2017). Dual language learners: A national demographic and policy profile. Migration Policy Institute.
  • Pew Research Center. (2012). The rise of Asian Americans. Pew Social & Demographic Trends. https://www.pewresearch.org/social-trends/2012/06/19/the-rise-of-asian-americans/
  • Raikes, H., Alexander Pan, B., Luze, G., Tamis LeMonda, C. S., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., Banks Tarullo, L., Abigail Raikes, H., & Rodriguez, E. T. (2006). Mother–child bookreading in low-income families: Correlates and outcomes during the first three years of life. Child Development, 77(4), 924–953. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00911.x
  • R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/
  • Rea-Sandin, G., Korous, K. M., & Causadias, J. M. (2021). A systematic review and meta-analysis of racial/ethnic differences and similarities in executive function performance in the United States. Neuropsychology, 35(2), 141–156. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000715
  • Reeves, R., Rodrigue, E., & Kneebone, E. (2016). Five evils: Multidimensional poverty and race in America. Brookings Institution.
  • Ribner, A. D., Tamis LeMonda, C. S., & Liben, L. S. (2020). Mothers’ distancing language relates to young children’s math and literacy skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 196, 104863. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2020.104863
  • Rodríguez, B. L., Hines, R., & Montiel, M. (2009). Mexican American mothers of low and middle socioeconomic status: Communication behaviors and interactive strategies during shared book reading. Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, 40(3), 271–282. https://doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/07-0053)
  • Roosa, M. W., Liu, F. F., Torres, M., Gonzales, N. A., Knight, G. P., & Saenz, D. (2008). Sampling and recruitment in studies of cultural influences on adjustment: A case study with Mexican Americans. Journal of Family Psychology, 22(2), 293–302. https://doi.org/10.1037/0893-3200.22.2.293
  • RStudio Team. (2021). RStudio: Integrated development environment for R. RStudio, PBC. http://www.rstudio.com/
  • Sénéchal, M. (1997). The differential effect of storybook reading on preschoolers’ acquisition of expressive and receptive vocabulary. Journal of Child Language, 24(1), 123–138. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000996003005
  • Sigel, I. E. (1993). The centrality of a distancing model for the development of representational competence. In R. R. Cocking & K. A. Renninger (Eds.), The development and meaning of psychological distance (pp. 141–158). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.
  • Sigel, I. E. (2002). The psychological distancing model: A study of the socialization of cognition. Culture & Psychology, 8(2), 189–214. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X02008002438
  • Steiner, P. M., Cook, T. D., Shadish, W. R., & Clark, M. H. (2010). The importance of covariate selection in controlling for selection bias in observational studies. Psychological Methods, 10, 1037. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018719
  • Swain, M. (2000). The output hypothesis and beyond: Mediating acquisition through collaborative dialogue. In J. P. Lantolf (Ed.), Sociocultural theory and second language learning (pp. 97–114). Oxford University Press.
  • Tompkins, V., Bengochea, A., Nicol, S., & Justice, L. M. (2017). Maternal inferential input and children’s language skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 52(4), 397–416. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.176
  • Uchikoshi, Y. (2014). Development of vocabulary in Spanish-speaking and Cantonese-speaking English language learners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(1), 119–153. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0142716412000264
  • U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2017). Policy statement on supporting the development of children who are dual language learners in early childhood programs. https://eclkc.ohs.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/pdf/dll-policy-statement-final.pdf
  • Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.
  • Wang, Q., Leichtman, M. D., & Davies, K. I. (2000). Sharing memories and telling stories: American and Chinese mothers and their 3-year-olds. Memory, 8(3), 159–177. https://doi.org/10.1080/096582100387588
  • Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Tominey, S. L., & Acock, A. C. (2011). The influence of demographic risk factors on children’s behavioral regulation in prekindergarten and kindergarten. Early Education and Development, 22(3), 461–488. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2011.536132
  • West, S. G., Finch, J. F., & Curran, P. J. (1995). Structural equation models with nonnormal variables: Problems and remedies. In R. H. Hoyle (Ed.), Structural equation modeling: Concepts, issues, and applications (pp. 56–75). Sage Publications, Inc.
  • White, L. J., Alexander, A., & Greenfield, D. B. (2017). The relationship between executive functioning and language: Examining vocabulary, syntax, and language learning in preschoolers attending head start. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 164, 16–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.06.010
  • White, L. J., & Greenfield, D. B. (2017). Executive functioning in Spanish- and English-speaking head start preschoolers. Developmental Science, 20(1), e12502. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12502
  • Whitehurst, G. J., Arnold, D. S., Epstein, J. N., Angell, A. L., Smith, M., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). A picture book reading intervention in day care and home for children from low-income families. Developmental Psychology, 30(5), 679–689. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.30.5.679
  • Willard, A. K., Busch, J. T. A., Cullum, K. A., Letourneau, S. M., Sobel, D. M., Callanan, M., & Legare, C. H. (2019). Explain this, explore that: A study of parent–child interaction in a children’s museum. Child Development, 90(5), e598–e617. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.13232
  • Williams, A. I., Uchikoshi, Y., Bunge, S. A., & Zhou, Q. (2019). Relations of English and heritage language proficiency to response inhibition and attention shifting in dual language learners in head start. Early Education & Development, 30(3), 357–374. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2018.1544812
  • Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., Greenberg, M., & The Family Life Project Investigators. (2010). The measurement of executive function at age 3 years: Psychometric properties and criterion validity of a new battery of tasks. Psychological Assessment, 22(2), 306–317.
  • Woodcock, R. W., & Muñoz-Sandoval, A. (1995). Woodcock language proficiency battery-revised, Spanish form. Riverside.
  • Xing, X., Liu, X., & Wang, M. (2019). Parental warmth and harsh discipline as mediators of the relations between family SES and Chinese preschooler’s inhibitory control. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 48, 237–245. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.12.018
  • Zelazo, P. D. (1999). Language, levels of consciousness, and the development of intentional action. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention: Social understanding and self-control (pp. 95–117). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2020). The neurodevelopment of executive function skills: Implications for academic achievement gaps. Psychology & Neuroscience, 13(3), 273–298. https://doi.org/10.1037/pne0000208
  • Zelazo, P. D., Müller, U., Frye, D., Marcovitch, S., Argitis, G., Boseovski, J., Chiang, J. K., Hongwanishkul, D., Schuster, B. V., Sutherland, A., & Carlson, S. M. (2003). The development of executive function in early childhood. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), i–151. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x
  • Zucker, T. A., Justice, L. M., Piasta, S. B., & Kaderavek, J. N. (2010). Preschool teachers’ literal and inferential questions and children’s responses during whole-class shared reading. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(1), 65–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2009.07.001

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.