1,234
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Public Support for COVID-19 Responses: Cultural Cognition, Risk Perception, and Emotions

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon

References

  • Bazzi, S., Fiszbein, M., & Gebresilasse, M. (2020). Rugged individualism and collective (in) action during the COVID-19 pandemic (NBER Working Paper No. 27776). National Bureau of Economic Research. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpubeco.2020.104357
  • Beall, J. M., Casola, W. R., Peterson, M. N., Larson, L. R., Carr, W. A., Seekamp, E., Stevenson, K. T., & Jackson, S. B. (2021). Cultural cognition and ideological framing influence communication about zoonotic disease in the era of COVID-19. Frontiers in Communication, 6. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2021.645692
  • Bian, B., Li, J., Xu, T., & Foutz, N. (2020). Individualism during crises: Big data analytics of collective actions amid COVID-19 (SSRN Scholarly Paper ID 3620364). Social Science Research Network. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn,3620364.
  • Bogel-Burroughs, N., & Peters, J. W. (2020, April 20). ‘You have to disobey’: Protesters gather to defy stay-at-home orders. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/2020/04/16/us/coronavirus-rules-protests.html
  • Böhm, G., & Pfister, H. R. (2017). The perceiver’s social role and a risk’s causal structure as determinants of environmental risk evaluation. Journal of Risk Research, 20(6), 732–759. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2015.1118148
  • Byrne, S., & Hart, P. S. (2009). The “boomerang” effect: A synthesis of findings and a preliminary theoretical framework. Annals of the International Communication Association, 33(1), 3–37. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2009.11679083
  • Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC). (2020, July 24). Health equity considerations and racial and ethnic minority groups. https://www.cdc.gov/coronavirus/2019-ncov/community/health-equity/race-ethnicity.html
  • Chadwick, A. (2015). Toward a theory of persuasive hope: Effects of cognitive appraisals, hope appeals, and hope in the context of climate change. Health Communication, 30(6), 598–611. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2014.916777
  • Chui, A. C., Titman, S., & Wei, K. J. (2010). Individualism and momentum around the world. The Journal of Finance, 65(1), 361–392. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.2009.01532.x
  • Della Cava, M. (2020, April 2). ‘This will blow over’: In states without stay-at-home orders, Americans celebrate freedom as death toll climbs. USA Today. https://www.usatoday.com/story/news/nation/2020/04/02/states-without-stay-home-orders-residents-celebrate-freedoms/5105303002/
  • Dimock, M., & Wike, R. (2020, November 13). America is exceptional in the nature of its political divide. Pew Research Center. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/11/13/america-is-exceptional-in-the-nature-of-its-political-divide/
  • Douglas, M., & Wildavsky, A. B. (1982). Risk and culture: An essay on the selection of technical and environmental dangers. University of California Press.
  • Dryhurst, S., Schneider, C. R., Kerr, J., Freeman, A. L. J., Recchia, G., van der Bles, A. M., Spiegelhalter, D., & Van Der Linden, S. (2020). Risk perceptions of COVID-19 around the world. Journal of Risk Research, 23(7–8), 994–1006. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1758193
  • Finucane, M., Alhakami, A., Slovic, P., & Johnson, S. (2000). The affect heuristic in judgments of risks and benefits. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 13(1), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0771(200001/03)13:1<1::AID-BDM333>3.0.CO;2-S
  • Germani, A., Buratta, L., Delvecchio, E., & Mazzeschi, C. (2020). Emerging adults and COVID-19: The role of individualism-collectivism on perceived risks and psychological maladjustment. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 17(10), 3497. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph17103497
  • Hayes, A. F. (2018). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis (2nd ed.). The Guilford Press.
  • Hofstede, G. (1993). Cultural constraints in management theories. Academy of Management Perspectives, 7(1), 81–94. https://doi.org/10.5465/ame.1993.9409142061
  • Ipsos. (2021, June 29). Americans aware of Delta coronavirus variant, but re-emergence continues. https://www.ipsos.com/en-us/news-polls/axios-ipsos-coronavirus-index?utm_source=newsletter&utm_medium=email&utm_campaign=newsletter_axiosam&stream=top
  • Ji, L. J., Zhang, Z., Usborne, E., & Guan, Y. (2004). Optimism across cultures: In response to the severe acute respiratory syndrome outbreak. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 7(1), 25–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467–839X.2004.00132.x
  • Jin, Y. (2010). Making sense sensibly in crisis communication: How publics’ crisis appraisals influence their negative emotions, coping strategy preferences, and crisis response acceptance. Communication Research, 37(4), 522–552. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650210368256
  • Johnson, B. B. (2017). Explaining Americans’ responses to dread epidemics: An illustration with Ebola in late 2014. Journal of Risk Research, 20(10), 1338–1357. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2016.1153507
  • Kahan, D. M. (2008). Two conceptions of emotion in risk regulation. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 156(3), 741–766. https://doi.org/10.2307/40041359
  • Kahan, D. M., & Braman, D. (2006). Cultural cognition and public policy. Yale Law & Policy Review, 24, 149–172.
  • Kahan, D. M., Braman, D., Cohen, G. L., Gastil, J., & Slovic, P. (2010). Who fears the HPV vaccine, who doesn’t, and why? An experimental study of the mechanisms of cultural cognition. Law and Human Behavior, 34(6), 501–516. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10979-009-9201-0
  • Kahan, D. M., Braman, D., Gastil, J., Slovic, P., & Mertz, C. (2007). Culture and identity‐protective cognition: Explaining the white‐male effect in risk perception. Journal of Empirical Legal Studies, 4(3), 465–505. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1740-1461.2007.00097.x
  • Kahan, D. M., Braman, D., Slovic, P., Gastil, J., & Cohen, G. (2008). Cultural cognition of the risks and benefits of nanotechnology. Nature Nanotechnology, 4(2), 87–90. https://doi.org/10.1038/nnano.2008.341
  • Kahan, D. M., Jamieson, K. H., Landrum, A., & Winneg, K. (2017). Culturally antagonistic memes and the Zika virus: An experimental test. Journal of Risk Research, 20(1), 1–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2016.1260631
  • Kahan, D. M., Jenkins-Smith, H., & Braman, D. (2011). Cultural cognition of scientific consensus. Journal of Risk Research, 14(2), 147–174. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2010.511246
  • Kasdan, D. O., & Campbell, J. W. (2020). Dataveillant collectivism and the coronavirus in Korea: Values, biases, and socio-cultural foundations of containment efforts. Administrative Theory & Praxis, 42(4), 604–613. https://doi.org/10.1080/10841806.2020.1805272
  • Kim, H. K., & Kim, Y. (2019). Risk information seeking and processing about particulate air pollution in South Korea: The roles of cultural worldview. Risk Analysis, 39(5), 1071–1087. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.13231
  • Kim, H. K., & Niederdeppe, J. (2013). The role of emotional response during an H1N1 influenza pandemic on a college campus. Journal of Public Relations Research, 25(1), 30–50. https://doi.org/10.1080/1062726X.2013.739100
  • Kim, S., & Kim, S. (2018). Exploring the determinants of perceived risk of middle East respiratory syndrome (MERS) in Korea. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health, 15(6), 1168. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph15061168
  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991a). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press.
  • Lazarus, R. S. (1991b). Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion. American Psychologist, 46(8), 819–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.46.8.819
  • Leiserowitz, A. (2006). Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and values. Climatic Change, 77(1), 45–72. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-006-9059-9
  • Lerner, J. S., Gonzalez, R. M., Small, D. A., & Fischhoff, B. (2003). Effects of fear and anger on perceived risks of terrorism: A national field experiment. Psychological Science, 14(8), 144–150. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.01433
  • Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 473–493. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402763
  • Li, J. B., Yang, A., Dou, K., Wang, L. X., Zhang, M. C., & Lin, X. Q. (2020). Chinese public’s knowledge, perceived severity, and perceived controllability of COVID-19 and their associations with emotional and behavioural reactions, social participation, and precautionary behaviour: A national survey. BMC Public Health, 20(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12889-020-09695-1
  • Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267–286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267
  • Lu, J. G., Jin, P., & English, A. S. (2021). Collectivism predicts mask use during COVID-19. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(23), e2021793118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2021793118
  • Lwin, M. O., Lu, J., Sheldenkar, A., Schulz, P. J., Shin, W., Gupta, R., & Yang, Y. (2020). Global sentiments surrounding the COVID-19 pandemic on Twitter: Analysis of Twitter trends. JMIR Public Health and Surveillance, 6(2), e19447. https://doi.org/10.2196/19447
  • Mervosh, S., Lu, D., & Swales, V. (2020, April 20). See which states and cities have told residents to stay at home. The New York Times. https://www.nytimes.com/interactive/2020/us/coronavirus-stay-at-home-order.html
  • Morss, R. E., Lazrus, H., Bostrom, A., & Demuth, J. L. (2020). The influence of cultural worldviews on people’s responses to hurricane risks and threat information. Journal of Risk Research, 23(12), 1620–1649. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1750456
  • Nabi, R. L. (2010). The case for emphasizing discrete emotions in communication research. Communication Monographs, 77(2), 153–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637751003790444
  • Nabi, R. L., Gustafson, A., & Jensen, R. (2018). Framing climate change: Exploring the role of emotion in generating advocacy behavior. Science Communication, 40(4), 442–468. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547018776019
  • Nabi, R. L., & Myrick, J. G. (2019). Uplifting fear appeals: Considering the role of hope in fear-based persuasive messages. Health Communication, 34(4), 463–474. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2017.1422847
  • Newman, T., Nisbet, E., & Nisbet, M. (2018). Climate change, cultural cognition, and media effects: Worldviews drive news selectivity, biased processing, and polarized attitudes. Public Understanding of Science, 27(8), 985–1002. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662518801170
  • O’Shea, B. A., & Ueda, M. (2021). Who is more likely to ignore experts’ advice related to COVID-19? Preventive Medicine Reports, 101470. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pmedr.2021.101470
  • Oh, S. H., Lee, S. Y., & Han, C. (2020). The effects of social media use on preventive behaviors during infectious disease outbreaks: The mediating role of self-relevant emotions and public risk perception. Health Communication, 36(8), 972–981. https://doi.org/10.1080/10410236.2020.1724639
  • Park, T., Ju, I., Ohs, J. E., & Hinsley, A. (2021). Optimistic bias and preventive behavioral engagement in the context of COVID-19. Research in Social and Administrative Pharmacy, 17(1), 1859–1866. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sapharm.2020.06.004
  • Rajkumar, R. P. (2021). The relationship between measures of individualism and collectivism and the impact of COVID-19 across nations. Public Health in Practice, 2, 100143. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.puhip.2021.100143
  • Roseman, I. J. (1991). Appraisal determinants of discrete emotions. Cognition & Emotion, 5(3), 161–200. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939108411034
  • Singal, J. (2020, May 8). The theory that explains the politicization of coronavirus. Intelligencer. https://nymag.com/intelligencer/2020/05/the-theory-that-explains-the-politicization-of-coronavirus.html
  • Slovic, P. (1987). Perception of risk. Science, 236(4799), 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507
  • Smith, C. A., & Kirby, L. D. (2001). Affect and cognitive appraisal: From content to process models. In J. Forgas (Ed.), Handbook of affect and social cognition (pp. 75–92). Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1990). Emotion and adaptation. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 609–637). Guilford.
  • Sun, Y. (2014). Rethinking public health: Promoting public engagement through a new discursive environment. American Journal of Public Health, 104(1), e6–e13. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2013.301638
  • Varma, M. M., Chen, D., Lin, X. L., Aknin, L. B., & Hu, X. (2020). Prosocial behavior promotes positive emotion during the COVID-19 pandemic. American Journal of Public Health, 104(1), e6–e13. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/vdw2e
  • Vartti, A. M., Oenema, A., Schreck, M., Uutela, A., de Zwart, O., Brug, J., & Aro, A. R. (2009). SARS knowledge, perceptions, and behaviors: A comparison between Finns and the Dutch during the SARS outbreak in 2003. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 16(1), 41–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12529-008-9004-6
  • Wallack, L., & Lawrence, R. (2005). Talking about public health: Developing America’s ‘second language’. American Journal of Public Health, 95(4), 567–570. https://doi.org/10.2105/ajph.2004.043844
  • Weinstein, N. (1989). Optimistic biases about personal risks. Science, 246(4935), 1232–1233. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.2686031
  • Wildavsky, A., & Dake, K. (1990). Theories of risk perception: Who fears what and why? Daedalus, 119(4), 41–60.
  • Wise, T., Zbozinek, T. D., Michelini, G., & Hagan, C. C. (2020). Changes in risk perception and protective behavior during the first week of the COVID-19 pandemic in the United States. Royal Society Open Science, 7(9), 200742. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/dz428
  • Witte, K. (1992). Putting the fear back into fear appeals: The extended parallel process model. Communication Monographs, 59(4), 329–349. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637759209376276
  • Xue, J., Chen, J., Hu, R., Chen, C., Zheng, C., Su, Y., & Zhu, T. (2020). Twitter discussions and emotions about the COVID-19 pandemic: Machine learning approach. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 22(11), e20550. https://doi.org/10.2196/20550
  • Xue, W., Hine, D. W., Loi, N. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Phillips, W. J. (2014). Cultural worldviews and environmental risk perceptions: A meta-analysis. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 40, 249–258. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2014.07.002
  • Yang, J. Z. (2016). Altruism during Ebola: Risk perception, issue salience, cultural cognition, and information processing. Risk Analysis, 36(6), 1079–1089. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12526
  • Yang, J. Z., & Chu, H. (2018). Who is afraid of the Ebola outbreak? The influence of discrete emotions on risk perception. Journal of Risk Research, 21(7), 834–853. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2016.1247378
  • Zhang, X. A. (2021). Understanding the cultural orientations of fear appeal variables: A cross-cultural comparison of pandemic risk perceptions, efficacy perceptions, and behaviors. Journal of Risk Research, 24(3–4), 432–448. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2021.1887326

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.