864
Views
62
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Applying processing trees in social psychology

&
Pages 116-159 | Received 21 Dec 2015, Accepted 12 Jul 2016, Published online: 05 Aug 2016

References

  • Arnold, N. R., Bayen, U. J., & Smith, R. E. (2015). Hierarchical multinomial modeling approaches: An application to prospective memory and working memory. Experimental Psychology, 62, 143–152. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000287
  • Bargh, J. A. (1994). The four horsemen of automaticity: Awareness, efficiency, intention, and control in social cognition. In R. S. Wyer Jr. & T. K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1–40). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Batchelder, W. H., & Alexander, G. E. (2013). Discrete-state models: Comment on Pazzaglia, Dube, and Rotello (2013). Psychological Bulletin, 139, 1204–1212. doi:10.1037/a0033894
  • Batchelder, W. H., & Riefer, D. M. (1999). Theoretical and empirical review of multinomial processing tree modeling. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 6, 57–86. doi:10.3758/BF03210812
  • Bayen, U. J., Murnane, K., & Erdfelder, E. (1996). Source discrimination, item detection, and multinomial models of source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 197–215.
  • Begg, I. M., Anas, A., & Farinacci, S. (1992). Dissociation of processes in belief: Source recollection, statement familiarity, and the illusion of truth. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 121, 446–458. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.121.4.446
  • Bishara, A. J., & Payne, B. K. (2009). Multinomial process tree models of control and automaticity in weapon misidentification. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 524–534. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.002
  • Blair, I. V., Ma, J. E., & Lenton, A. P. (2001). Imagining stereotypes away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 828–841. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.5.828
  • Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., & Vaterrodt-Plünnecke, B. (1995). Toward unbiased measurement of conscious and unconscious memory processes within the process dissociation framework. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 137–160. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.137
  • Burke, C. T. (2015). Process dissociation models in racial bias research: Updating the analytic method and integrating with signal detection approaches. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 18, 402–434. doi:10.1177/1368430214567763
  • Busemeyer, J. R., & Wang, Y. (2000). Model comparisons and model selections based on the generalization criterion methodology. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 44, 171–189. doi:10.1006/jmps.1999.1282
  • Calanchini, J., Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J., & Klauer, C. (2013). Counter-prejudicial training reduces activation of biased associations and enhances response monitoring. European Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 321–325. doi:10.1002/ejsp.v43.5
  • Calanchini, J., Sherman, J. W., Klauer, K. C., & Lai, C. K. (2014). Attitudinal and non-attitudinal components of IAT performance. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 1285–1296. doi:10.1177/0146167214540723
  • Campbell, D. X., & Fiske, D. W. (1959). Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait-multimethod matrix. Psychological Bulletin, 56, 81–105. doi:10.1037/h0046016
  • Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Hugenberg, K., & Groom, C. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The quad-model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469–487. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.469
  • Conway, P., & Gawronski, B. (2013). Deontological and utilitarian inclinations in moral decision-making: A process dissociation approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 216–235. doi:10.1037/a0031021
  • Curran, T., & Hintzman, D. L. (1995). Violations of the independence assumption in process dissociation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23, 531–547.
  • De Houwer, J. (2003). The extrinsic affective Simon task. Experimental Psychology, 50, 77–85. doi:10.1026//1618-3169.50.2.77
  • De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an alternative for association formation models. Learning & Behavior, 37, 1–20. doi:10.3758/LB.37.1.1
  • Erdfelder, E. (2000). Multinomiale Modelle in der kognitiven Psychologie [Multinomial models in cognitive psychology] ( Unpublished habilitation thesis). Psychologisches Institut der Universitat Bonn, Germany.
  • Erdfelder, E., Auer, T.-S., Hilbig, B. E., Assfalg, A., Moshagen, M., & Nadarevic, L. (2009). Multinomial processing tree models: A review of the literature. Journal of Psychology, 217, 108–124.
  • Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., & Williams, C. J. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial-attitudes – A bona-fide pipeline. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 1031–1027. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013
  • Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2003). Implicit measures in social cognition research: Their meaning and use. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 297–327. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145225
  • Ferreira, M. B., Garcia-Marques, L., Sherman, S. J., & Sherman, J. W. (2006). Automatic and controlled components of judgment and decision making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91, 797–813. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.797
  • Fiedler, K., & Bluemke, M. (2005). Faking the IAT: Aided and unaided response control on the Implicit Association Test. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 307–316. doi:10.1207/s15324834basp2704_3
  • Fiedler, K., & Freytag, P. (2004). Pseudocontingencies. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 453–467. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.4.453
  • Gaunt, R., Leyens, J., & Demoulin, S. (2002). Intergroup relations and the attribution of emotions: Control over memory for secondary emotions associated with the ingroup and outgroup. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38, 508–514. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00014-8
  • Gawronski, B., Balas, R., & Creighton, L. A. (2014). Can the formation of conditioned attitudes be intentionally controlled?. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40, 419–432. doi:10.1177/0146167213513907
  • Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2006). Associative and propositional processes in evaluation: An integrative review of implicit and explicit attitude change. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 692–731. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.5.692
  • Gawronski, B., Conway, P., Armstrong, J., Friesdorf, R., & Hütter, M. (2016). Understanding responses to moral dilemmas: Deontological inclinations, utilitarian inclinations, and general action tendencies. In J. P. Forgas, L. Jussim, & P. A. M. Van Lange (Eds.), Social psychology of morality (pp. 91–110). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., Allen, T. J., Klauer, K. C., & Amodio, D. M. (2011). Accounting for successful control of implicit racial bias: The roles of association activation, response monitoring, and overcoming bias. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1534–1545. doi:10.1177/0146167211414064
  • Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., & Klauer, K. C. (2009). Aging and prejudice: Diminished regulation of automatic race bias among older adults. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 410–414. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.004
  • Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J. W., & Klauer, K. C. (2014). Measures of implicit attitudes may conceal differences in implicit associations: The case of anti-aging bias. Social and Personality Psychology Science, 5, 271–278. doi:10.1177/1948550613499239
  • Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral Judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108. doi:10.1126/science.1062872
  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464
  • Hamilton, D. L., & Gifford, R. K. (1976). Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 12, 392–407. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(76)80006-6
  • Heathcote, A., Brown, S. D., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015). An introduction to good practices in cognitive modeling. In B. U. Forstmann & E.-J. Wagenmakers (Eds.), An introduction to model-based cognitive neuroscience (pp. 25–48). New York, NY: Springer.
  • Hense, R. L., Penner, L. A., & Nelson, D. L. (1995). Implicit memory for age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 13, 399–415. doi:10.1521/soco.1995.13.4.399
  • Hu, X., & Batchelder, W. H. (1994). The statistical analysis of general processing tree models with the EM algorithm. Psychometrika, 59, 21–47. doi:10.1007/BF02294263
  • Hütter, M., & Sweldens, S. (2013). Implicit misattribution of evaluative responses: Contingency-unaware evaluative conditioning requires simultaneous stimulus presentations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 638–643. doi:10.1037/a0029989
  • Hütter, M., Sweldens, S., Stahl, C., Unkelbach, C., & Klauer, K. C. (2012). Dissociating contingency awareness and conditioned attitudes: Evidence of contingency-unaware evaluative conditioning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141, 539–557. doi:10.1037/a0026477
  • Ito, T. A., Friedman, N. P., Bartholow, B. D., Correll, J., Loersch, C., Altamirano, L. J., & Miyake, A. (2015). Toward a comprehensive understanding of executive cognitive function in implicit racial bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(2), 187–218. doi:10.1037/a0038557
  • Jacoby, L. L. (1991). A process dissociation framework: Separating automatic from intentional uses of memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 513–541. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(91)90025-F
  • Jones, C. R., Fazio, R. H., & Olson, M. A. (2009). Implicit misattribution as a mechanism underlying evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 933–948. doi:10.1037/a0014747
  • Judd, C., Westfall, J., & Kenny, D. A. (2012). Treating stimuli as a random factor in social psychology: A new and comprehensive solution to a pervasive but largely ignored problem. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 54–69. doi:10.1037/a0028347
  • Klauer, K. C. (2006). Hierarchical multinomial processing tree models: A latent-class approach. Psychometrika, 71, 1–31. doi:10.1007/s11336-004-1188-3
  • Klauer, K. C. (2010). Hierarchical multinomial processing tree models: A latent-trait approach. Psychometrika, 75, 70–98. doi:10.1007/s11336-009-9141-0
  • Klauer, K. C. (2015). Mathematical modeling. In B. Gawronksi & G. Bodenhausen (Eds.), Theory and explanation in social psychology (pp. 371–389). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Klauer, K. C., Dittrich, K., Scholtes, C., & Voss, A. (2015). The invariance assumption in process-dissociation models: An evaluation across three domains. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 198–221. doi:10.1037/xge0000044
  • Klauer, K. C., & Ehrenberg, K. (2005). Social categorization and fit detection under cognitive load: Efficient or effortful?. European Journal of Social Psychology, 35, 493–516. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992
  • Klauer, K. C., Ehrenberg, K., & Wegener, I. (2003). Crossed categorization and stereotyping: Structural analyses, effect patterns, and dissociative effects of context relevance. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 332–354. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00017-9
  • Klauer, K. C., Hölzenbein, F., Calanchini, J., & Sherman, J. (2014). How malleable is categorization by race? Evidence for competitive category use in social categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 107, 21–40. doi:10.1037/a0036609
  • Klauer, K. C., & Kellen, D. (2015). The flexibility of models of recognition memory: The case of confidence ratings. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 67, 8–25. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2015.05.002
  • Klauer, K. C., & Meiser, T. (2000). A source-monitoring analysis of illusory correlations. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1074–1093. doi:10.1177/01461672002611005
  • Klauer, K. C., Schmitz, F., Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Voss, A. (2010). Understanding the role of executive control in the implicit association test: Why flexible people have small IAT effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 595–619. doi:10.1080/17470210903076826
  • Klauer, K. C., Stahl, C., & Voss, A. (2011). Multinomial models and diffusion models. In K. C. Klauer, A. Voss, & C. Stahl (Eds.), Cognitive methods in social psychology (pp. 367–390). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Klauer, K. C., & Wegener, I. (1998). Unraveling social categorization in the “Who said what?” paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 1155–1178. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.75.5.1155
  • Klauer, K. C., Wegener, I., & Ehrenberg, K. (2002). Perceiving minority members as individuals: The effects of relative group size in social categorization. European Journal of Social Psychology, 32, 223–245. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992
  • Krieglmeyer, R., & Sherman, J. W. (2012). Disentangling stereotype activation and stereotype application in the Stereotype Misperception Task. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103, 205–224. doi:10.1037/a0028764
  • Kurzban, R., Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can race be erased? Coalitional computation and social categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 15387–15392. doi:10.1073/pnas.251541498
  • Lindsay, D. S., & Jacoby, L. L. (1994). Stroop process dissociations: The relationship between facilitation and interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 20, 219–234.
  • Mangat, N. (1994). An improved randomized-response strategy. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B, 56, 93–95.
  • Matzke, D., Dolan, C. V., Batchelder, W. H., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015). Bayesian estimation of multinomial processing tree models with heterogeneity in participants and items. Psychometrika, 80, 205–235. doi:10.1007/s11336-013-9374-9
  • McGarty, C., & de la Haye, A.-M. (1997). Stereotype formation: Beyond illusory correlation. In R. Spears, P. J. Oakes, N. Ellemers, & S. A. Haslam (Eds.), The social psychology of stereotyping and group life (pp. 144–170). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.
  • Meiser, T., & Hewstone, M. (2001). Crossed categorization effects on the formation of illusory correlations. European Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 443–466. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992
  • Meiser, T., & Hewstone, M. (2004). Cognitive processes in stereotype formation: The role of correct contingency learning for biased group judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 599–614. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.599
  • Meiser, T., & Hewstone, M. (2006). Illusory and spurious correlations: Distinct phenomena or joint outcomes of exemplar-based category learning?. European Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 315–336. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992
  • Meissner, F., & Rothermund, K. (2013). Estimating the contributions of associations and recoding in the Implicit Association Test: The ReAL model for the IAT. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 104, 45–69. doi:10.1037/a0030734
  • Meissner, F., & Rothermund, K. (2015). The insect-nonword IAT revisited: Dissociating between evaluative associations and recoding. Social Psychology, 46, 46–54. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000220
  • Mierke, J., & Klauer, K. C. (2001). Implicit association measurement with the IAT: Evidence for effects of executive control processes. Experimental Psychology, 48, 107–122. doi:10.1026//0949-3946.48.2.107
  • Mierke, J., & Klauer, K. C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1180–1192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1180
  • Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183–198. doi:10.1017/S0140525X09000855
  • Moons, W. G., Mackie, D. M., & Garcia-Marques, T. (2009). The impact of repetition-induced familiarity on agreement with weak and strong arguments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 32–44. doi:10.1037/a0013461
  • Moshagen, M. (2010). multiTree: A computer program for the analysis of multinomial processing tree models. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 42–54. doi:10.3758/BRM.42.1.42
  • Moshagen, M., Musch, J., & Erdfelder, E. (2012). A stochastic lie detector. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 222–231. doi:10.3758/s13428-011-0144-2
  • Nadarevic, L., & Erdfelder, E. (2011). Cognitive processes in implicit attitude tasks: An experimental validation of the Trip Model. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 254–268. doi:10.1002/ejsp.776
  • Nosek, B. A., Banaji, M., & Greenwald, A. G. (2002). Harvesting implicit group attitudes and beliefs from a demonstration web site. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 6, 101–115. doi:10.1037/1089-2699.6.1.101
  • Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2001). The go/no-go association task. Social Cognition, 19, 625–666. doi:10.1521/soco.2001.19.issue-6
  • Payne, B. K. (2001). Prejudice and perception: The role of automatic and controlled processes in misperceiving a weapon. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 181–192. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.81.2.181
  • Payne, B. K., & Bishara, A. J. (2009). An integrative review of process dissociation and related models in social cognition. European Review of Social Psychology, 20, 272–314. doi:10.1080/10463280903162177
  • Payne, B. K., Burkley, M. A., & Stokes, M. B. (2008). Why do implicit and explicit attitude tests diverge? The role of structural fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 16–31. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.16
  • Payne, B. K., Jacoby, L. L., & Lambert, A. J. (2004). Memory monitoring and the control of stereotype distortion. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 52–64. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00069-6
  • Pitt, M. A., Myung, I. J., & Zhang, S. (2002). Toward a method of selecting among computational models of cognition. Psychological Review, 109, 472–491. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.3.472
  • Pleyers, G., Corneille, O., Luminet, O., & Yzerbyt, V. (2007). Aware and (dis)liking: Item-based analyses reveal that valence acquisition via evaluative conditioning emerges only when there is contingency awareness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory & Cognition, 33, 130–144.
  • Rakić, T., Steffens, M. C., & Mummendey, A. (2011). Blinded by the accent! The minor role of looks in ethnic categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 16–29. doi:10.1037/a0021522
  • Riefer, D. M., & Batchelder, W. H. (1988). Multinomial modeling and the measurement of cognitive processes. Psychological Review, 95, 318–339. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.3.318
  • Riefer, D. M., Hu, X., & Batchelder, W. H. (1994). Response strategies in source monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 680–693.
  • Rothermund, K., Teige-Mocigemba, S., Gast, A., & Wentura, D. (2009). Minimizing the influence of recoding in the implicit association test: The recoding-free implicit association test (IAT-RF). The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 84–98. doi:10.1080/17470210701822975
  • Rouder, J. N., Lu, J., Morey, R. D., Sun, D., & Speckman, P. L. (2008). A hierarchical process-dissociation model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 370–398. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.370
  • Rubin, D. C., & Wenzel, A. E. (1996). One hundred years of forgetting: A quantitative description of retention. Psychological Review, 103, 734–760. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.4.734
  • Schug, J., Alt, N. P., & Klauer, K. C. (2015). Gendered race prototypes: Evidence for the non-prototypicality of Asian men and Black women. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 56, 121–125. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2014.09.012
  • Schwarz, N., & Clore, G. L. (1983). Mood, misattribution, and judgments of well-being: Informative and directive functions of affective states. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 513–523. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.45.3.513
  • Self, S. G., & Liang, K.-Y. (1987). Asymptotic properties of maximum likelihood estimators and likelihood ratio tests under nonstandard conditions. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 82, 605–611. doi:10.1080/01621459.1987.10478472
  • Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., Gonsalkorale, K., Hugenberg, K., Allen, T. J., & Groom, C. J. (2008). The self-regulation of automatic associations and behavioral impulses. Psychological Review, 115, 314–335. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.115.2.314
  • Sherman, J. W., Groom, C. J., Ehrenberg, K., & Klauer, K. C. (2003). Bearing false witness under pressure: Implicit and explicit components of stereotype-driven memory distortions. Social Cognition, 21, 213–246. doi:10.1521/soco.21.3.213.25340
  • Singmann, H., & Kellen, D. (2013). MPTinR: Analysis of multinomial processing tree models in R. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 560–575. doi:10.3758/s13428-012-0259-0
  • Smith, J. B., & Batchelder, W. H. (2008). Assessing individual differences in categorical data. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 713–731. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.4.713
  • Stahl, C. (2006). Multinomiale Verarbeitungsbaummodelle in der Sozialpsychologie [Multinomial processing tree models in social psychology]. Zeitschrift Für Sozialpsychologie, 37, 161–171. doi:10.1024/0044-3514.37.3.161
  • Stahl, C., & Degner, J. (2007). Assessing automatic activation of valence: A multinomial model of EAST performance. Experimental Psychology, 54, 99–112. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.54.2.99
  • Stahl, C., & Klauer, K. C. (2007). HMMTree: A computer program for hierarchical multinomial processing tree models. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 267–273. doi:10.3758/BF03193157
  • Stahl, C., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). A simplified conjoint recognition paradigm for the measurement of gist and verbatim memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 570–586.
  • Stahl, C., Unkelbach, C., & Corneille, O. (2009). On the respective contributions of awareness of US valence and US identity in attitude formation through evaluative conditioning. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 404–420. doi:10.1037/a0016196
  • Steffens, M. C. (2004). Is the implicit association test immune to faking?. Experimental Psychology, 51, 165–179. doi:10.1027/1618-3169.51.3.165
  • Strack, F., & Deutsch, R. (2004). Reflective and impulsive determinants of social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 8, 220–247. doi:10.1207/pspr.2004.8.issue-3
  • Taylor, S., Fiske, S., Etcoff, N., & Ruderman, A. (1978). Categorical and contextual bases of person memory and stereotyping. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 36, 778–793. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.36.7.778
  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., & Klauer, K. C. (2008). ‘Automatic’ evaluation? Strategic effects on affective priming. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1414–1417. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2008.04.004
  • Teige-Mocigemba, S., Klauer, K. C., & Rothermund, K. (2008). Minimizing method-specific variance in the IAT: A Single Block IAT. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 24, 237–245.
  • Van Zandt, T. (2002). Analysis of response time distributions. In J. T. Wixted (Vol. Ed.) & H. Pashler (Series Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology (3rd Ed.), Vol. 4: Methodology in Experimental Psychology (pp. 461–512). New York, NY: John Wiley.
  • Wallaert, M., Ward, A., & Mann, T. (2010). Explicit control of implicit responses: Simple directives can alter IAT performance. Social Psychology, 41, 152–157. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000022
  • Wegener, I., & Klauer, K. C. (2004). Inter-category versus intra-category fit: When social categories match social context. European Journal of Social Psychology, 34, 567–593. doi:10.1002/(ISSN)1099-0992
  • Wilson, T., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101–126. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101
  • Wu, H., Myung, J. I., & Batchelder, W. H. (2010a). Minimum description length model selection of multinomial processing tree models. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17, 275–286. doi:10.3758/PBR.17.3.275
  • Wu, H., Myung, J. I., & Batchelder, W. H. (2010b). On the minimum description length complexity of multinomial processing tree models. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 291–303. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2010.02.001

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.