397
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research

Influence of Religion on Prosociality: A Priming Study of Buddhist Concepts among Nonbelievers in China

ORCID Icon, , , , &

References

  • Ahmed, A. M. (2009). Are religious people more prosocial? A quasi‐experimental study with madrasah pupils in a rural community in India. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 48(2), 368–374. doi:10.1111/jssr.2009.48.issue-2
  • Ajzen, I., & Fishbein, M. (1977). Attitude-behavior relations: A theoretical analysis and review of empirical research. Psychological Bulletin, 84(5), 888. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.84.5.888
  • Anderson, L. R., & Mellor, J. M. (2009). Religion and cooperation in a public goods experiment. Economics Letters, 105(1), 58–60. doi:10.1016/j.econlet.2009.05.016
  • Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind in the middle: A Practical Guide to Priming and Automaticity Research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 253–285). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Bateson, M., Nettle, D., & Roberts, G. (2006). Cues of being watched enhance cooperation in a real-world setting. Biology Letters, 2(3), 412–414. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0509
  • Ben-Ner, A., & Halldorsson, F. (2010). Trusting and trustworthiness: What are they, how to measure them, and what affects them. Journal of Economic Psychology, 31(1), 64–79. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2009.10.001
  • Bering, J. (2012). The belief instinct: The psychology of souls, destiny, and the meaning of life. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company.
  • Castelli, F., Happe, F., Frith, U., & Frith, C. (2000). Movement and mind: A functional imaging study of perception and interpretation of complex intentional movement patterns. Neuroimage, 12, 314–325. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0612
  • Chasteen, A. L., Burdzy, D. C., & Pratt, J. (2010). Thinking of God moves attention. Neuropsychologia, 48(2), 627–630. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.09.029
  • Clobert, M., & Saroglou, V. (2013). Intercultural non-conscious influences: Prosocial effects of Buddhist priming on Westerners of Christian tradition. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 37(4), 459–466. doi:10.1016/j.ijintrel.2012.10.001
  • Clobert, M., Saroglou, V., & Hwang, K. K. (2015). Buddhist concepts as implicitly reducing prejudice and increasing prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 41(4), 513–525. doi:10.1177/0146167215571094
  • Colzato, L. S., van Beest, I., van Den Wildenberg, W. P., Scorolli, C., Dorchin, S., Meiran, N., & Hommel, B. (2010). God: Do I have your attention? Cognition, 117(1), 87–94. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.07.003
  • Colzato, L. S., van Den Wildenberg, W. P., & Hommel, B. (2008). Losing the big picture: How religion may control visual attention. PLoS One, 3(11), e3679. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0003679
  • Davidson, R. J., & Harrington, A. (Eds.). (2002). Visions of compassion: Western scientists and Tibetan Buddhists examine human nature. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Fishbach, A., Friedman, R. S., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2003). Leading us not into temptation: Momentary allurements elicit overriding goal activation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 296. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.296
  • Galen, L. W. (2012). Does religious belief promote prosociality? A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 138(5), 876. doi:10.1037/a0028251
  • Gervais, W. M., & Norenzayan, A. (2012). Like a camera in the sky? Thinking about God increases public self-awareness and socially desirable responding. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 48(1), 298–302. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2011.09.006
  • Gervais, W. M., Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2011). Do you believe in atheists? Distrust is central to anti-atheist prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(6), 1189. doi:10.1037/a0025882
  • Gilder, T. S., & Heerey, E. A. (2018). The role of experimenter belief in social priming. Psychological Science, 29(3), 403–417. doi:10.1177/0956797617737128
  • Goodman, C. (2009). Consequences of compassion: An interpretation and defense of Buddhist ethics. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Haley, K. J., & Fessler, D. M. (2005). Nobody’s watching? Subtle cues affect generosity in an anonymous economic game. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(3), 245–256. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2005.01.002
  • Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., & Lesorogol, C. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327(5972), 1480–1484. doi:10.1126/science.1182238
  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). Beyond WEIRD: Towards a broad-based behavioral science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 111–135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X10000725
  • Johnson, K. A., Li, Y. J., Cohen, A. B., & Okun, M. A. (2013). Friends in high places: The influence of authoritarian and benevolent god-concepts on social attitudes and behaviors. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 5(1), 15. doi:10.1037/a0030138
  • Kacen, J. J., & Lee, J. A. (2002). The influence of culture on consumer impulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 163–176. doi:10.1207/S15327663JCP1202_08
  • Koole, S. L., McCullough, M. E., Kuhl, J., & Roelofsma, P. H. (2010). Why religion’s burdens are light: From religiosity to implicit self-regulation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 95–107. doi:10.1177/1088868309351109
  • Laurin, K., Kay, A. C., & Fitzsimons, G. M. (2012). Divergent effects of activating thoughts of God on self-regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(1), 4. doi:10.1037/a0025971
  • Ma-Kellams, C., Spencer-Rodgers, J., & Peng, K. (2011). I am against us? Unpacking cultural differences in ingroup favoritism via dialecticism. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(1), 15–27.
  • Neely, J. H., & Kahan, T. A. (2001). Is semantic activation automatic? In H. L. Roediger III, J. S. Nairne, I. Neath, & A. M. Surprenant (Eds.), The nature of remembering: Essays in honor of Robert G. Crowder (pp. 117–150). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
  • Norenzayan, A., Henrich, J., & Slingerland, E. (2013). Religious prosociality: A synthesis. In P. J. Richerson & M. H. Christiansen (Eds.), Cultural Evolution: Society, Technology, Language and Religion (pp. 365–378). Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Pichon, I., Boccato, G., & Saroglou, V. (2007). Nonconscious influences of religion on prosociality: A priming study. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(5), 1032–1045. doi:10.1002/ejsp.v37:5
  • Prakash, G. (2018). Buddhist Attitude towards Sustainable Development Postawa buddyjska a zrównoważony rozwój. PROBLEMY EKOROZWOJU, 13(1), 217–220.
  • Preston, J. L., Ritter, R. S., & Ivan Hernandez, J. (2010). Principles of religious prosociality: A review and reformulation. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 4(8), 574–590. doi:10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00286.x
  • Ramsay, J. E., Pang, J. S., Shen, M. J., & Rowatt, W. C. (2014). Rethinking value violation: Priming religion increases prejudice in Singaporean Christians and Buddhists. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 24(1), 1–15. doi:10.1080/10508619.2012.761525
  • Ramsay, J. E., Tong, E. M., Pang, J. S., & Chowdhury, A. (2016). A puzzle unsolved: Failure to observe different effects of God and religion primes on intergroup attitudes. PloS one, 11(1), e0147178. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0147178
  • Saroglou, V., Delpierre, V., & Dernelle, R. (2004). Values and religiosity: A meta-analysis of studies using Schwartz’s model. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(4), 721–734. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2003.10.005
  • Sasaki, J. Y., Kim, H. S., Mojaverian, T., Kelley, L. D., Park, I. Y., & Janušonis, S. (2011). Religion priming differentially increases prosocial behavior among variants of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(2), 209–215. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr089
  • Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 199–207. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn050
  • Shah, J. Y., Friedman, R., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Forgetting all else: On the antecedents and consequences of goal shielding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(6), 1261. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1261
  • Shah, J. Y., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2002). Priming against your will: How accessible alternatives affect goal pursuit. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(4), 368–383. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00005-7
  • Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–809. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01983.x
  • Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Religious priming: A meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(1), 27–48. doi:10.1177/1088868314568811
  • Van Cappellen, P., Corneille, O., Cols, S., & Saroglou, V. (2011). Beyond mere compliance to authoritative figures: Religious priming increases conformity to informational influence among submissive people. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 21(2), 97–105. doi:10.1080/10508619.2011.556995
  • Wenger, J. L. (2004). The automatic activation of religious concepts: Implications for religious orientations. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 14(2), 109–123. doi:10.1207/s15327582ijpr1402_3

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.