418
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Can you keep a secret? Introducing the RT-based Concealed Information Test to children

, , &
Pages 276-301 | Received 29 May 2014, Accepted 07 Oct 2015, Published online: 17 Nov 2015

References

  • Achenbach, T. M. (1985). Assessment and taxonomy of child and adolescent psychopathology. Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.
  • Ahern, E. C., Lyon, T. D., & Quas, J. A. (2011). Young children's emerging ability to make false statements. Developmental Psychology, 47(1), 61–66. doi:10.1037/a0021272
  • Alloway, T. P., McCallum, F., Alloway, R. G., & Hoicka, E. (2015). Liar, liar, working memory on fire: Investigating the role of working memory in childhood verbal deception. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 137, 30–38. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.03.013
  • Ambach, W., Stark, R., Peper, M., & Vaitl, D. (2008). An interfering go/no-go task does not affect accuracy in a concealed information test. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 68, 6–16. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.12.007
  • Anderson, P. (2002). Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 8, 71–82. doi:10.1076/chin.8.2.71.8724
  • Ben-Shakhar, G. (1985). Standardization within individuals: A simple method to neutralize individual differences in skin conductance. Psychophysiology, 22(3), 292–299. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.1985.tb01603.x
  • Ben-Shakhar, G. (2012). Current research and potential applications of the concealed information test: An overview. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 342. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00342
  • Ben-Shakhar, G., & Elaad, E. (2003). The validity of psychophysiological detection of information with the Guilty Knowledge Test: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(1), 131. doi:10.1037/0021–9010.88.1.131
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. doi:10.1111/j.1467–8624.2010.01499.x
  • Bond, G. D. (2012). Focus on basic cognitive mechanisms and strategies in deception research (and remand custody of ‘wizards’ Harry Potter movies). Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(1), 128–130. doi: 10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.04.003
  • Bush, L. K., Hess, U., & Wolford, G. (1993). Transformations for within-subject designs: A Monte Carlo investigation. Psychological bulletin, 113(3), 566. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.566
  • Cambridge Cognition (2006). Cambridge neuropsychological test automated battery (CANTABeclipse). Cambridge: Cambridge Cognition Ltd.
  • Carlson, S. M., & Moses, L. J. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children's theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00333
  • Carlson, S. M., Moses, L. J., & Hix, H. (1998). The role of inhibitory processes in young children's difficulties with deception and false belief. Child Development, 69, 672–69. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1998.tb06236.x
  • Carmel, D., Dayan, E., Naveh, A., Raveh, O., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2003). Estimating the validity of the guilty knowledge test from simulated experiments: The external validity of mock crime studies. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 261–269. doi:10.1037/1076-898X.9.4.261
  • Chorpita, B. F., Moffitt, C. E., & Gray, J. (2005). Psychometric properties of the Revised Child Anxiety and Depression Scale in a clinical sample. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(3), 309–322. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.02.004
  • Christ, S. E., Van Essen, D. C., Watson, J. M., Brubaker, L. E., & McDermott, K. B. (2009). The contributions of prefrontal cortex and executive control to deception: Evidence from activation likelihood estimate meta-analyses. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1557–1566. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhn189
  • Cutmore, T. R. H., Djakovic, T., Kebbell, M. R., & Shum, D. H. (2009). An object cue is more effective than a word in ERP-based detection of deception. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 71, 185–192. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.08.003
  • Debey, E., De Schryver, M., Logan, G. D., Suchotzki, K., & Verschuere, B. (2015). From junior to senior Pinocchio: A cross-sectional lifespan investigation of deception. Acta psychologica, 160, 58–68. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.06.007
  • Debey, E., Liefooghe, B., De Houwer, J., & Verschuere, B. (2014). Lie, truth, lie: The role of task switching in a deception context. Psychological Research, 1–11. doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0582-4
  • Debey, E., Verschuere, B., & Crombez, G. (2012). Lying and executive con troll: An experimental investigation using ego depletion and goal neglect. Acta Psychologica, 140, 133–141. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.03.004
  • Ennis, E., Vrij, A., & Chance, C. (2008). Individual differences and lying in everyday life. Journal of Social and Personal Relations, 25, 105–118. doi:10.1177/0265407507086808
  • Evans, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Verbal deceptions from late childhood to middle adolescence and its relation to executive functioning skills. Developmental Psychology, 49, 39–49. doi:10.1037/a0023425
  • Evans, A. D., & Lee, K. (2013). Emergence of lying in very young children. Developmental Psychology, 49(10), 1958–1963. doi:10.1037/a0031409
  • Evans, A., Xu, F., & Lee, K. (2011). When all signs point to you lies told in the face of evidence. Developmental Psychology, 47(1), 39–49. doi:10.1037/a0020787
  • Farrow, T. F., Hopwood, M. C., Parks, R. W., Hunter, M. D., & Spence, S. A. (2010). Evidence of mnemonic ability selectively affecting truthful and deceptive response dynamics. The American Journal of Psychology, 4, 447–453. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/10.5406/amerjpsyc.123.4.0447
  • Faust, M. E., Balota, D. A., Spieler, D. H., & Ferraro, F. R. (1999). Individual differences in information-processing rate and amount: Implications for group differences in response latency. Psychological Bulletin, 125(6), 777. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.125.6.777
  • Fischer, C. B. (2005). Deception research involving children: Ethical practices and paradoxes. Ethics & Behavior, 15(3), 271–287. doi:10.1207/s15327019eb1503_7
  • Gamer, M., & Ambach, W. (2014). Deception research today. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 256. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00256
  • Gamer, M., Bauermann, T., Stoeter, P., & Vossel, G. (2007). Covariations among fMRI, skin conductance and behavioral data during processing of concealed information. Human Brain Mapping, 28, 1287–1301. doi:10.1002/hbm.20343
  • Gamer, M., Kosiol, D., & Vossel, G. (2010). Strength of memory encoding affects physiological responses in the Guilty Actions Test. Biological Psychology, 83(2), 101–107. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.005
  • Giesen, M., & Rollison, M. A. (1980). Guilty knowledge versus innocent associations: Effects of trait anxiety and stimulus context on skin conductance. Journal of Research in Personality, 14, 1–11. doi:10.1016/0092-6566(80)90035-5
  • Gombos, V. A. (2006). The cognition of deception: The role of executive processes in producing lies. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 132(3), 197–214. doi:10.3200/MONO.132.3.197-214
  • Hu, X., & Rosenfeld, J. P. (2012). Combining the P300-complex trial-based Concealed Information Test and the reaction time-based autobiographical Implicit Association Test in concealed memory detection. Psychophysiology, 49(8), 1090–1100. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01389.x
  • Hu, X., Evans, A., Wu, H., Lee, K., & Fu, G. (2013). An interfering dot-probe task facilitates the detection of mock crime memory in a reaction time (RT) – based concealed information test. Acta Psychologica, 142, 278–285. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.12.006
  • Hughes, C., & Ensor, R. (2011). Individual differences in growth in executive function across the transition to school predict externalizing and internalizing behaviors and self-perceived academic success at 6 years of age. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 663–676. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.06.005
  • Ingram, E. M. (1994). Effects of electrodermal lability and anxiety on the detection of deception with the control question technique. Report No. DoDPI94-R-0004. Department of Defense Polygraph Institute, Ft. McClellan, AL 36205.
  • Kensinger, E. A. (2007). Negative emotion enhances memory accuracy behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(4), 213–218. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00506.x
  • Kiselev, S., Espy, K. A., & Sheffield, T. (2009). Age-related differences in reaction time task performance in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102(2), 150–166. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.02.002
  • Korkman, M., Kirk, U., & Kemp, S. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Kozel, F. A., Johnson, K. A., Mu, Q., Grenesko, E. L., Laken, S. J., & George, M. S. (2005). Detecting deception using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Biological Psychiatry, 58, 605–613. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.040
  • Lee, K. (2013). Little liars: Development of verbal deception in children. Child Development Perspectives, 7(2), 91–96. doi:10.1111/cdep.12023
  • Lieblich, I. (1969). Manipulation of contrast between differential GSR responses through the use of ordered tasks of information detection. Psychophysiology, 6, 70–77. doi:10.1111/j.1469–8986.1969.tb02885.x
  • Lieblich, I. (1970). Manipulation of contrast between differential GSRs in very young children. Psychophysiology, 7(3), 436–441. doi:10.1111/j.1469–8986.1970.tb01769.x
  • Lieblich, I., Kugelmass, S., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1970). Efficiency of GSR detection of information as a function of stimulus set size. Psychophysiology, 6, 601–608. doi:10.1111/j.1469–8986.1970.tb02249.x
  • Luciana, M., & Nelson, C. A. (1998). The functional emergence of prefrontally-guided working memory systems in four- to eight-year-old children. Neuropsychologia, 36(3), 273–293. doi:10.1016/S0028–3932(97)00109–7
  • Lykken, D. T. (1959). The GSR in the detection of guilt. Journal of Applied Psychology, 43, 385–388. doi:10.1037/h0046060
  • Meijer, E. H., Smulders, F. T., Merckelbach, H. L., & Wolf, A. G. (2007). The P300 is sensitive to concealed face recognition. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 66(3), 231–237. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.08.001
  • Melinder, A., Goodman, G. S., Eilertsen, D. E., & Magnussen, S. (2004). Beliefs about child witnesses: A survey of professionals. Psychology, Crime & Law, 10(4), 347–365. doi:10.1080/10683160310001618717
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wagner, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ‘frontal lobe’ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Nahari, G., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (2011). Psychophysiological and behavioral measures for detecting concealed information: The role of memory for crime details. Psychophysiology, 48(6), 733–744. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01148.x
  • Ng, E. L., & Lee, K. (2010). Children's task performance under stress and non-stress conditions. Cognition & Emotion, 24(7), 1229–1238. doi:10.1080/02699930903172328
  • Ostrov, J. M., Ries, E. E., Stauffacher, K., Godleski, S. A., & Mullins, A. D. (2008). Relational Aggression, physical aggression and deception during early childhood: A multimethod, multiinformant short-term longitudinal study. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 37, 664–675. doi:10.1080/15374410802148137
  • Peskin, J. (1992). Ruse and representations: On children's ability to conceal information. Developmental Psychology, 28, 84–89. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.1.84
  • Reynolds, C. R. (2004). Behavior assessment system for children. In Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology (pp. 1–2). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. doi:10.1002/9780470479216.corpsy0114
  • Salekin, R. T., Kubak, F. A., & Lee, Z. (2008). Deception in children and adolescents. In R. Rogers (Ed.) Clinical assessment of malingering and deception (3rd ed., pp. 343–364). New York, NY: Guilford Press. Retrieved from: http://psycnet.apa.org/index.cfm?fa=search.displayRecord&UID=2008-09622-019
  • Seymour, T. L., & Kerlin, J. R. (2008). Successful detection of verbal and visual concealed knowledge using an RT-based paradigm. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 475–490. doi:10.1002/acp.1375
  • Seymour, T. L., Seifert, C. M., Shafto, M. G., & Mosmann, A. L. (2000). Using response time measures to assess ‘guilty knowledge’. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85, 30–37. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.85.1.30
  • Talwar, V., & Crossman, A. (2011). From little white lies to filthy liars: The evolution of honesty and deception in young children. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 40, 139–179. doi:10.1016/B978-0-12-386491-8.00004-9
  • Talwar, V., Gordon, H., & Lee, K. (2007). Lying in the elementary school: Verbal deception and its relation to second-order belief understanding. Developmental Psychology, 43, 804–810. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.3.804
  • Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2008). Social and cognitive correlates of children's lying behavior. Child Development, 79, 866–881. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01164
  • Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2002a). Emergence of white lie-telling in children between 3 and 7 years of age. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(2), 160–181. doi:10.1353/mpq.2002.0009
  • Talwar, V., & Lee, K. (2002b). Development of lying to conceal a transgression: Children's control of expressive behaviour during verbal deception. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 26, 436–444. doi:10.1080/01650250143000373
  • Talwar, V., Lee, K., Bala, N., & Lindsay, R. C. L. (2002). Children's conceptual knowledge of lie-telling and its relation to their actual behaviors: Implications for court competence examination. Law and Human Behavior, 26, 395–415. doi:10.1023/A:1016379104959
  • Varga, M., Visu-Petra, G., & Miclea, M. (2015). Sequencing the RT-based Concealed Information Test: Contributions from individual differences in working memory and processing speed. Manuscript submitted for publication.
  • Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Degrootte, T., & Rosseel, Y. (2009). Detecting concealed information with reaction times: Validity and comparison with the polygraph. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1–11. doi:10.1002/acp.1601
  • Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., Koster, E. H. W., Van Bockstaele, B., & De Clercq, A. (2007). Startling secrets: Startle eye blink modulation by concealed crime information. Biological Psychology, 76, 52–60. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.06.001
  • Verschuere, B., Rosenfeld, J. P., Winograd, M., Labkovsky, E., & Wiersema, R. (2009). The role of deception in P300 memory detection. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 14, 253–262. doi:10.1348/135532508X384184
  • Visu-Petra, L., Cheie, L., Benga, O., Alloway, T. P. (2011). Effects of trait anxiety on memory storage and updating in young children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 35, 38–47. doi:10.1177/0165025410368945
  • Visu-Petra, G., Miclea, M., Buş, I., & Visu-Petra, L. (2014). Detecting concealed information: The role of individual differences in executive functions and social desirability. Psychology, Crime & Law, 20(1), 1–7. doi:10.1080/1068316X.2012.736509
  • Visu-Petra, G., Miclea, M., & Visu-Petra, L. (2012). RT-based detection of concealed information in relation to individual differences in executive functioning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 342–351. doi:10.1002/acp.1827
  • Visu-Petra, G., Varga, M., Miclea, M., & Visu-Petra, L. (2013). When interference helps, increasing executive load to facilitate deception detection in the consealed information test. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 146. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00146
  • Vrij, A., & Holland, M. (1998). Individual differences in persistence in lying and experiences while deceiving. Communication Research Reports, 15(3), 299–308. doi:10.1080/08824099809362126
  • Vrij, A., Mann, S., Fisher, R., Leal, S., Milne, B., & Bull, R. (2008). Increasing cognitive load to facilitate lie detection: The benefit of recalling an event in reverse order. Law and Human Behavior, 32, 253–265. doi:10.1007/s10979–007–9103-y
  • Walczyk, J. J., Roper, K. S., Seemann, E., & Humphrey, A. M. (2003). Cognitive mechanisms underlying lying to questions: Response time as a cue to deception. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 17, 755–774. doi:10.1002/acp.914
  • Wasserman, S., & Bockenholt, U. (1989). Bootstrapping: Applications to psychophysiology. Psychophysiology, 26, 208–221. doi:10.1111/j.1469–8986.1989.tb03159.x
  • Watson, A., & Valtin, R. (1997). Secrecy in middle childhood. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 431. doi:10.1080/016502597384730
  • Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler intelligence scale for children–Fourth Edition (WISC-IV). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Williams, S., Moore, K., Crossman, A. M., & Talwar, V. (2015). The role of executive functions and theory of mind in children's prosocial lie-telling. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.08.001

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.