1,056
Views
13
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The Theory of Cognitive Spacetime

REFERENCES

  • Altmann, G. T. M. (2004). Language-mediated eye movements in the absence of a visual world: The “blank screen paradigm.” Cognition, 93, B79–B87.
  • Beech, J. R. (1980). Imaginal vs. perceptual scanning of a visual representation. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 50, 367–370.
  • Bennett, B., Cohn, A. G., Wolter, F., & Zakharyaschev, M. (2002). Multi-dimensional modal logic as a framework for spatio-temporal reasoning. Applied Intelligence, 17, 239–251.
  • Bergen, B. K., & Chan Lau, T. T. (2012). Writing direction affects how people map space onto time. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–5.
  • Boroditsky, L. (2000). Metaphoric structuring: Understanding time through spatial metaphors. Cognition, 75, 1–28.
  • Boroditsky, L., & Gaby, A. (2010). Remembrances of times east: Absolute spatial representations of time in an Australian aboriginal community. Psychological Science, 21, 1635–1639.
  • Boroditsky, L., & Ramscar, M. (2002). The roles of body and mind in abstract thought. Psychological Science, 13, 185–189.
  • Borst, G., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2008). Visual mental imagery and visual perception: Structural equivalence revealed by scanning processes. Memory & Cognition, 36, 849–862.
  • Boyes Braem, P. (1990). Einführung in die Gebärdensprache und ihre Erforschung [Introduction to sign language and sign language research] (3rd.ed.). Germany: Signum-Verlag.
  • Brandt, S.A., & Stark, L. W. (1997). Spontaneous eye movements during visual imagery reflect the content of the visual scene. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9, 27–38.
  • Brennan, M. (1983). Marking time in British sign language. In J. Kyle & B. Woll (Eds.), Language in sign: An international perspective on sign language (pp. 10–31). London, England: Croom Helm.
  • Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). Moving through imagined space: Mentally simulating locomotion during spatial description reading. Acta Psychologica, 134, 110–124.
  • Cacciari, C., Bolognini, N., Senna, I., Pellicciari, M., Miniussi, C., & Papagno, C. (2011). Literal, fictive and metaphorical motion sentences preserve the motion component of the verb: A TMS study. Brain and Language, 119, 149–157.
  • Cacciari, C., & Pesciarelli, F. (2013). Motor activation in literal and non literal sentences: Does time matter? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 1–10.
  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593.
  • Casasanto, D., Fotakopoulou, O., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Space and time in the child’s mind: Evidence for a cross-dimensional asymmetry. Cognitive Science, 34, 387–405.
  • Clark, H. H. (1973). Space, time, semantics, and the child. In T. E. Moore (Ed.), Cognitive development and the acquisition of language (pp. 27–63). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Cohn, A. G, Magee, D. R., Galata, A., Hogg, D. C., & Hazarika, S. M. (2003). Towards an architecture for cognitive vision using qualitative spatio-temporal representations and abduction. In C. Freksa, W. Brauer, C. Habel, & K. F. Wender (Eds.), Spatial cognition III (pp. 232–248). Berlin. Germany: Springer.
  • Damasio, A. R., Grabowski, T. J., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Ponto, L. L. B., Parvizi, J., & Hichwa, R. D. (2000). Subcortical and cortical brain activity during the feeling of self-generated emotions. Nature Neuroscience, 3, 1049–1056.
  • De Vos, C. L. G. (2012). Sign-spatiality in Kata Kolok: How a village sign language of Bali inscribes its signing space (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen, Nijmegen, The Netherlands. Retrieved from http://repository.ubn.ru.nl/bitstream/2066/99153/1/99153.pdf
  • Demarais, A. M., & Cohen, B. H. (1998). Evidence for image-scanning eye movements during transitive inference. Biological Psychology, 49, 229–247.
  • Denis, M., & Cocude, M. (1989). Scanning visual images generated from verbal descriptions. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 1, 293–307.
  • Dolcos, F., LaBar, K. S., & Cabeza, R. (2005). Remembering one year later: Role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102, 2626–2631.
  • Dollár, P., Rabaud, V., Cottrell, G., & Belongie, S. (2005). Behavior recognition via sparse spatio-temporal features. Proceedings of the 2nd Joint IEEE International Workshop on Visual Surveillance and Performance Evaluation of Tracking and Surveillance (pp. 65–72). Beijing, China: IEEE Computer Society.
  • Egeth, H. E., & Yantis, S. (1997). Visual attention: Control, representation, and time course. Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 269–297.
  • Ehrich. V, & Koster, C. (1983). Discourse organization and sentence form: The structure of room descriptions in Dutch. Discourse Processes, 6, 169–195.
  • Eich, E., Nelson, A. L., Leghari, M. A., & Handy, T. C. (2009). Neural systems mediating field and observer memories. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2239–2251.
  • Engberg-Pedersen, E. (1999). Space and time. In J. Allwood & P. Gärdenfors (Eds.), Cognitive semantics: Meaning and cognition (pp. 131–152). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
  • Evans, V. (2013). Language and Time: A Cognitive Linguistics Approach. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  • Frishberg, N., & Gough, B. (2000). Morphology in American Sign Language. Sign Language & Linguistics, 3(1), 103–132. ( Original work published 1973)
  • Galton, A. (2011). Time flies but space doesn’t: Limits to the spatialization of time. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 695–703.
  • Gentner, D., Imai, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2002). As time goes by: Evidence for two systems in processing space time metaphors. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 537–565.
  • Gibbs, R. W. (2006). Metaphor interpretation as embodied simulation. Mind & Language, 21, 434–458.
  • Hartmann, M., & Mast, F. W. (2012). Moving along the mental time line influences the processing of future related words. Consciousness and Cognition, 21, 1558–1562.
  • Henri, V., & Henri, C. (1896). Enquete sur les premier souvenirs de l’enfance [First survey on childhood memories]. L’Annee Psychologique, 3, 184–198.
  • Holsanova, J., Johansson, R., & Holmqvist, K. (2008). To tell and to show: The interplay of language and visualizations in communication. In P. Gärdenfors & A. Wallin (Eds.), A smorgasbord of cognitive science (pp. 215–229). Nora, Sweden: Nya Doxa.
  • Intons-Peterson, M. J. (1983). Imagery paradigms: How vulnerable are they to experimenters’ expectations? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 394–412.
  • Inui, T., Kawato, M., & Suzuki, R. (1978). The mechanism of mental scanning in foveal vision. Biological Cybernetics, 30, 147–155.
  • Jackendoff, R. (1987). On beyond zebra: The relation of linguistic and visual information. Cognition, 26, 89–114.
  • Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., Dewhurst, R., & Holmqvist, K. (2012). Eye movements during scene recollection have a functional role, but they are not reinstatements of those produced during encoding. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 289–1314.
  • Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., & Holmqvist, K. (2005). What do eye movements reveal about mental imagery? Evidence from visual and verbal elicitations. In B. G. Bara, L. Barsalou, & M. Bucciarelli (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1054–1059). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., & Holmqvist, K. (2006). Pictures and spoken descriptions elicit similar eye movements during mental imagery, both in light and in complete darkness. Cognitive Science, 30, 1053–1079.
  • Johansson, R., Holsanova, J., & Holmqvist, K. (2013). Using eye movements and spoken discourse as windows to inner space. In C. Paradis, J. Hudson, & U. Magnusson (Eds.), The construal of spatial meaning: Windows into conceptual space (pp. 9–28). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Jolicoeur, P., & Kosslyn, S. M. (1985). Is time to scan visual images due to demand characteristics? Memory & Cognition, 13, 320–332.
  • Kant, I. (2009). Kritik der reinen Vernunft [Critique of pure reason] (2nd ed.). Cologne, Germany: Anaconda. (Original work published 1787)
  • Kaschak, M., Madden, C., Therriault, D., Yaxley, R., Aveyard, M., Blanchard, A., & Zwaan, R. (2005). Perception of motion affects language processing. Cognition, 94, B79–B89.
  • Kendon, A. (1993). Space, time and gesture. Degrès, 74, 3–16.
  • Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Klippel, A., Wallgrün, J. O., Yang, J., Li, R., & Dylla, F. (2012). Formally grounding spatio-temporal thinking. Cognitive Processing, 13, S209–S214.
  • Laeng, B., Okubo, M., Saneyoshi, A., & Michimata, C. (2011). Processing spatial relations with different apertures of attention. Cognitive Science, 35, 297–329.
  • Laeng, B., & Teodorescu, D. S. (2002). Eye scanpaths during visual imagery reenact those of perception of the same visual scene. Cognitive Science, 26, 207–231.
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Le Guen, O. (2012). An exploration in the domain of time: From Yucatec Maya time gestures to Yucatec Maya Sign Language time signs. In U. Zeshan & C. de Vos (Eds.), Sign languages in village communities: Anthropological and linguistic insights (pp. 209–250). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Le Guen, O., & Pool Balam, L. I. (2012). No metaphorical timeline in gesture and cognition among Yucatec Mayas. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 1–15.
  • Levelt, W. J. M. (1982). Cognitive styles in the use of spatial direction terms. In R. J. Javella & W. Klein (Eds.), Speech, place, and action: Studies of deixis and related topics (pp. 251–268). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Linde, C., & Labov, W. (1975). Spatial networks as a site for the study of language and thought. Language, 51, 924–939.
  • Marr, D., & Nishihara, H. K. (1978). Representation and recognition of the spatial organization of three-dimensional shapes. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London, B200, 269–294.
  • Martarelli, C. S., & Mast, F. W. (2011). Preschool children’s eye-movements during pictorial recall. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 29, 425–436.
  • Martarelli, C. S., & Mast, F. W. (2013). Eye movements during long-term pictorial recall. Psychological Research, 77, 303–309.
  • Matlock, T. (2004). Fictive motion as cognitive simulation. Memory & Cognition, 32, 1389–1400.
  • Matlock, T. (2006). Depicting fictive motion in drawings. In J. Luchenbroers (Ed.), Cognitive linguistics investigations: Across languages, fields, and philosophical boundaries (pp. 67–85). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: John Benjamins.
  • Matlock, T., Ramscar, M., & Boroditsky, L. (2005). On the experiential link between spatial and temporal language. Cognitive Science, 29, 655–664.
  • Matlock, T., & Richardson, D. C. (2004). Do eye movements go with fictive motion? Proceedings of the 26th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 909–914). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • McGlone, M. S. & Harding, J. L. (1998). Back (or forward?) to the future: The role of perspective in temporal language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 1211–1223.
  • McTaggart, J. (1908). The unreality of time. Mind, 17, 457–474.
  • Meir, I., & Sandler, W. (2008). A language in space: The story of Israeli Sign Language. New York, NY: Erlbaum.
  • Miles, L., Betka, E,. Pendry, L., & Macrae, C. (2010). Mapping temporal constructs: Actions reveal that time is a place. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 2113–2119.
  • Miles, L., Karpinska, K., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C. (2010). The meandering mind: Vection and mental time travel. PLoS One, 5, e10825.
  • Miles, L., Nind, L., & Macrae, C. (2010). Moving through time. Psychological Science, 21, 222–223.
  • Miles, L., Tan, L., Noble, G., Lumsden, J., & Macrae, C. (2011). Can a mind have two time lines? Exploring space–time mapping in Mandarin and English speakers. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 598–604.
  • Mishra, R., & Singh, N. (2010). Online fictive motion understanding: An eye-movement study with Hindi. Metaphor and Symbol, 25, 144–161.
  • Moore, K. E. (2000). Spatial experience and temporal metaphors in Wolof: Point of view, conceptual mapping, and linguistic practice (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, CA.
  • Moore, K. E. (2006). Space-to-time mappings and temporal concepts. Cognitive Linguistics, 17, 199–244.
  • Moore, K. E. (2011). Ego-perspective and field-based frames of reference: Temporal meanings of front in Japanese, Wolof, and Aymara. Journal of Pragmatics, 43, 759–776.
  • Nigro, G., & Neisser, U. (1983). Point of view in personal memories. Cognitive Psychology, 15, 467–482.
  • Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K., Doan, D., & Wassmann, J. (2012). Contours of time: Topographic construals of past, present, and future in the Yupno valley of Papua New Guinea. Cognition, 124, 25–35.
  • Núñez, R. E., Motz, B. A., & Teuscher, U. (2006). Time after time: The psychological reality of the ego- and time-reference-point distinction in metaphorical construals of time. Metaphor and Symbol, 21, 133–146.
  • Núñez, R. E., & Sweetser, E. (2006). With the future behind them: Convergent evidence from Aymara language and gesture in the crosslinguistic comparison of spatial construals of time. Cognitive Science, 30, 401–450.
  • O’Keefe, J. (1996). The spatial prepositions in English, vector grammar, and the cognitive map theory. In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 277–316). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • O’Keefe, J. (2003). Vector grammar, places, and the functional role of spatial prepositions in English. In E. Van der Zee & J. Slack (Eds.), Representing direction in language and space (pp. 69–85). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Pinker, S. (1989). Learnability and cognition: The acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Pinker, S. (2007). The stuff of thought: Language as a window into human nature. New York, NY: Viking.
  • Ramscar, M., Matlock, T., & Dye, M. (2010). Running down the clock: The role of expectation in our understanding of time and motion. Language and Cognitive Processes, 25, 589–615.
  • Rice, H., & Rubin, D. (2009). I can see it both ways: First- and third-person visual perspectives at retrieval. Consciousness and Cognition, 18, 877–890.
  • Rice, H., & Rubin, D. (2011). Remembering from any angle: The flexibility of visual perspective during retrieval. Consciousness and Cognition, 20, 568–577.
  • Richardson, D. C., & Matlock, T. (2007). The integration of figurative language and static depictions: An eye movement study of fictive motion. Cognition, 102, 129–138.
  • Sakai, K., & Inui, T. (2002). A feature-segmentation model of short-term visual memory. Perception, 31, 579–589.
  • Saygin, A., McCullough, S., Alac, M., & Emmorey, K. (2010). Modulation of BOLD response in motion-sensitive lateral temporal cortex by real and fictive motion sentences. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2480–2490.
  • Sinha, C., Sinha, V., Zinken, J., & Sampaio, W. (2011) When time is not space: The social and linguistic construction of time intervals and temporal event relations in an Amazonian culture. Language and Cognition, 3, 137–169.
  • Stocker, K. (2012a). The time machine in our mind. Cognitive Science, 36, 385–420.
  • Stocker, K. (2012b). Toward an embodiment-disembodiment taxonomy. Cognitive Processing, 13, S347–S350.
  • Stocker, K. (2013a). Complex mental time travel. In M. Knauff, M. Pauen, N. Sebanz, & I. Wachsmuth (Eds.), Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3468–3473). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Stocker, K. (2013b). Digital causal cognition. International Journal of Cognitive Linguistics, 4, 9–34.
  • Talmy, L. (1996). Fictive motion in language and “caption.” In P. Bloom, M. A. Peterson, & M. F. Garrett (Eds.), Language and space (pp. 211–276). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Talmy, L. (2000a). Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 1: Concept structuring systems. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Talmy, L. (2000b). Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 2: Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Talmy, L. (2003). Recombinance in the evolution of language. In J. Cihlar, A. Franklin, D. Kaise, & I. Kimbara (Eds.), Proceedings of the 39th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society (pp. 33–66). Chicago, IL: Chicago Linguistic Society.
  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B. (1992). Spatial mental models derived from survey and route descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 261–292.
  • Taylor, H. A., & Tversky, B (1996). Perspective in spatial descriptions. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 371–391.
  • Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009). Covert shifts of attention function as an implicit aid to insight. Cognition, 111, 168–174.
  • Tversky, B., & Kugelmass, S., & Winter, A. (1991). Cross-cultural and developmental trends in graphic productions. Cognitive Psychology, 2, 515–557.
  • Ulrich, R., Eikmeier, V., de la Vega, I., Ruiz Fernández, S., Alex-Ruf, S., & Maienborn, C. (2012). With the past behind and the future ahead: Back-to-front representation of past and future sentences. Memory & Cognition, 40, 483–495.
  • Ulrich, R., & Maienborn, C. (2010). Left–right coding of past and future in language: The mental timeline during sentence processing. Cognition, 117, 126–138.
  • Wallentin, M., Østergaard, S., Lund, T., Østergaard, L., & Roepstorff, A. (2005). Concrete spatial language: See what I mean? Brain and Language, 92, 221–233.
  • Witkin, A., & Kass, M. (1988). Spacetime constraints. In SIGGRAPH ’88: Proceedings of the 15th Annual Conference on Computer Graphics and Interactive Techniques (pp. 159–168). New York, NY: ACM Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.