317
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Reward priming of temporal preparation

&
Pages 25-40 | Received 31 May 2014, Accepted 10 Dec 2014, Published online: 04 Feb 2015

REFERENCES

  • Anderson, B. A., Laurent, P. A., & Yantis, S. (2011). Value-driven attentional capture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 10367–10371. doi:10.1073/pnas.1104047108
  • Bargh, J. A. (1989). Conditional automaticity: Varieties of automatic influence in social perception and cognition. Unintended Thought, 3, 51–69.
  • Baumeister, A. A., & Joubert, C. E. (1969). Interactive effects on reaction time of preparatory interval length and preparatory interval frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 82, 393. doi:10.1037/h0028119
  • Berridge, K. C., & Robinson, T. E. (1998). What is the role of dopamine in reward: Hedonic impact, reward learning, or incentive salience? Brain Research Reviews, 28, 309–369. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00019-8
  • Braem, S., Hickey, C., Duthoo, W., & Notebaert, W. (2014). Reward determines the context-sensitivity of cognitive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 1769.
  • Braem, S., Verguts, T., Roggeman, C., & Notebaert, W. (2012). Reward modulates adaptations to conflict. Cognition, 125, 324–332. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.07.015
  • Chelazzi, L., Perlato, A., Santandrea, E., & Della Libera, C. (2013). Rewards teach visual selective attention. Vision Research, 85, 58–72. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2012.12.005
  • Correa, Á., Lupiáñez, J., Madrid, E., & Tudela, P. (2006). Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials. Brain Research, 1076(1), 116–128. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.11.074
  • Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1998). Where and when to pay attention: The neural systems for directing attention to spatial locations and to time intervals as revealed by both PET and fMRI. The Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 7426–7435.
  • Cousineau, D. (2005). Confidence intervals in within-subject designs: A simpler solution to Loftus and Masson's method. Tutorial in Quantitative Methodology in Psychology, 1(1), 4–45.
  • Della Libera, C., & Chelazzi, L. (2006). Visual selective attention and the effects of monetary rewards. Psychological Science, 17, 222–227.
  • Della Libera, C., & Chelazzi, L. (2009). Learning to attend and to ignore is a matter of gains and losses. Psychological Science, 20, 778–784. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02360x
  • Drazin, D. H. (1961). Effects of foreperiod, foreperiod variability, and probability of stimulus occurrence on simple reaction time. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 43–50. doi:10.1037/h0046860
  • Fiorillo, C. D., Tobler, P. N., & Schultz, W. (2003). Discrete coding of reward probability and uncertainty by dopamine neurons. Science, 299, 1898–1902. doi:10.1126/science.1077349
  • Hickey, C., Chelazzi, L., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). Reward changes salience in human vision via the anterior cingulate. The Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 11096–11103. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1026-10.2010
  • Hickey, C., Chelazzi, L., & Theeuwes, J. (2014). Reward-priming of location in visual search. PLoS One, 9, e103372. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0103372.g003
  • Hickey, C., Kaiser, D., & Peelen, M. V. (2015). Reward guides attention to object categories in real-world scenes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144. doi:10.1037/a0038627
  • Hickey, C., & van Zoest, W. (2012). Reward creates oculomotor salience. Current Biology, 22, R219–R220.
  • Hickey, C., & van Zoest, W. (2013). Reward-Associated stimuli capture the eyes in spite of strategic attentional set. Vision Research, 92, 67–74. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2013.09.008
  • Ikemoto, S., & Panksepp, J. (1999). The role of nucleus accumbens dopamine in motivated behavior: A unifying interpretation with special reference to reward-seeking. Brain Research Reviews, 31(1), 6–41. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(99)00023-5
  • Janssen, P., & Shadlen, M. N. (2005). A representation of the hazard rate of elapsed time in macaque area LIP. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 234–241. doi:10.1038/nn1386
  • Kristjánsson, Á., Eyjólfsdóttir, K. Ó., Jónsdóttir, A., & Arnkelsson, G. B. (2010). Temporal consistency is currency in shifts of transient attention. PLoS One, 5, e13660.
  • Kristjánsson, A., Sigurjónsdóttir, O., & Driver, J. (2010). Fortune and reversals of fortune in visual search: Reward contingencies for pop-out targets affect search efficiency and target repetition effects. Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 1229–1236.
  • Leuthold, H., Sommer, W., & Ulrich, R. (2004). Preparing for action: Inferences from CNV and LRP. Journal of Psychophysiology, 18(2), 77–88.
  • Los, S. A. (2013). The role of response inhibition in temporal preparation: Evidence from a go/no-go task. Cognition, 129, 328–344. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2013.07.013
  • Los, S. A., & Heslenfeld, D. J. (2005). Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation: Electrophysiological evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 134(1), 52. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.134.1.52
  • Los, S. A., Knol, D. L., & Boers, R. M. (2001). The foreperiod effect revisited: Conditioning as a basis for nonspecific preparation. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 121–145. doi:10.1016/S0001-6918(00)00029-9
  • Los, S. A., & Van Den Heuvel, C. E. (2001). Intentional and unintentional contributions to nonspecific preparation during reaction time foreperiods. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 370. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.27.2.370
  • Miniussi, C., Wilding, E. L., Coull, J. T., & Nobre, A. C. (1999). Orienting attention in time: Modulation of brain potentials. Brain, 122, 1507–1518. doi:10.1093/brain/122.8.1507
  • Moyer, J. R., Deyo, R. A., & Disterhoft, J. F. (1990). Hippocampectomy disrupts trace eye-blink conditioning in rabbits. Behavioral Neuroscience, 104, 243. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.104.2.243
  • Näätänen, R. (1971). Non-aging fore-period and simple reaction-time. Acta Psychologica, 35, 316–327.
  • Niemi, P., & Näätänen, R. (1981). Foreperiod and simple reaction time. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 133–162. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.89.1.133
  • Possamaï, C. A., Granjon, M., Reynard, G., & Requin, J. (1975). High order sequential effects and the negative gradient of the relationship between simple reaction-time and foreperiod duration. Acta Psychologica, 39, 263–270.
  • Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron, 36, 241–263. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00967-4
  • Shuler, M. G., & Bear, M. F. (2006). Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science, 311, 1606–1609. doi:10.1126/science.1123513
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2009). Dynamic adjustment of temporal preparation: Shifting warning signal modality attenuates the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 132(1), 40–47. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.06.002
  • Steinborn, M. B., Rolke, B., Bratzke, D., & Ulrich, R. (2010). The effect of a cross-trial shift of auditory warning signals on the sequential foreperiod effect. Acta Psychologica, 134, 94–104. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2009.12.011
  • Toates, F. (1986). Motivational systems. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Vallesi, A., & Shallice, T. (2007). Developmental dissociations of preparation over time: Deconstructing the variable foreperiod phenomena. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33, 1377. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1377
  • Vangkilde, S., Petersen, A., & Bundesen, C. (2013). Temporal expectancy in the context of a theory of visual attention. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368, 20130054.
  • Woodrow, H. (1914). The measurement of attention. Psychological Monographs, 17(5), 1–158.
  • Yashar, A., & Lamy, D. (2010). Intertrial repetition facilitates selection in time: Common mechanisms underlie spatial and temporal search. Psychological Science, 21, 243–251.
  • Yashar, A., & Lamy, D. (2013). Temporal position priming: Memory traces of recent experience bias the allocation of attention in time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 39, 1443–1456.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.