304
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Straight gaze facilitates face processing but does not cause involuntary attentional capture

, , &
Pages 381-391 | Received 18 Jul 2016, Accepted 05 Jan 2017, Published online: 16 Feb 2017

References

  • Anderson, D. E., Vogel, E. K., & Awh, E. (2011). Precision in visual working memory reaches a stable plateau when individual item limits are exceeded. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(3), 1128–1138. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4125-10.2011
  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that Can read minds - cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Cahiers De Psychologie Cognitive-Current Psychology of Cognition, 13(5), 513–552.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., Grant, J., & Walker, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 13(4), 379–398. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1995.tb00687.x
  • Burra, N., Barras, C., Coll, S. Y., & Kerzel, D. (2016). Electrophysiological evidence for attentional capture by irrelevant angry facial expressions. Biological Psychology, 120, 69–80. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.08.008
  • Burton, A. M., Bindemann, M., Langton, S. R., Schweinberger, S. R., & Jenkins, R. (2009). Gaze perception requires focused attention: Evidence from an interference task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 108–118. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.108
  • Calder, A. J., Jenkins, R., Cassel, A., & Clifford, C. W. (2008). Visual representation of eye gaze is coded by a nonopponent multichannel system. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 244–261. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.244
  • Cline, M. G. (1967). The perception of where a person is looking. The American Journal of Psychology, 80(1), 41–50. doi: 10.2307/1420539
  • Conty, L., Gimmig, D., Belletier, C., George, N., & Huguet, P. (2010). The cost of being watched: Stroop interference increases under concomitant eye contact. Cognition, 115(1), 133–139. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.12.005
  • Conty, L., N'Diaye, K., Tijus, C., & George, N. (2007). When eye creates the contact! ERP evidence for early dissociation between direct and averted gaze motion processing. Neuropsychologia, 45(13), 3024–3037. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.017
  • Conty, L., Tijus, C., Hugueville, L., Coelho, E., & George, N. (2006). Searching for asymmetries in the detection of gaze contact versus averted gaze under different head views: A behavioural study. Spatial Vision, 19(6), 529–545. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1163/156856806779194026
  • Cooper, R. M., Law, A. S., & Langton, S. R. (2013). Looking back at the stare-in-the-crowd effect: Staring eyes do not capture attention in visual search. Journal of Vision, 13(6), 11–22. doi: 10.1167/13.6.10
  • Doi, H., & Shinohara, K. (2013). Task-irrelevant direct gaze facilitates visual search for deviant facial expression. Visual Cognition, 21(1), 72–98. doi:10.1080/13506285.2013.779350
  • Doi, H., & Ueda, K. (2007). Searching for a perceived stare in the crowd. Perception, 36(5), 773–780. doi: 10.1068/p5614
  • Doi, H., Ueda, K., & Shinohara, K. (2009). Neural correlates of the stare-in-the-crowd effect. Neuropsychologia, 47(4), 1053–1060. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.004
  • Eimer, M. (1996). The N2pc component as an indicator of attentional selectivity. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 99(3), 225–234. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(96)95711-9
  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. H. (2002). Eye contact detection in humans from birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(14), 9602–9605. doi:10.1073/pnas.152159999
  • George, N., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Seen gaze-direction modulates fusiform activity and its coupling with other brain areas during face processing. NeuroImage, 13(6 Pt 1), 1102–1112. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0769
  • von Grünau, M., & Anston, C. (1995). The detection of gaze direction: A stare-in-the-crowd effect. Perception, 24(11), 1297–1313. doi: 10.1068/p241297
  • Johnson, M. H., Dziurawiec, S., Ellis, H., & Morton, J. (1991). Newborns’ preferential tracking of face-like stimuli and its subsequent decline. Cognition, 40(1–2), 1–19. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(91)90045-6
  • Johnson, M. H., Senju, A., & Tomalski, P. (2015). The two-process theory of face processing: Modifications based on two decades of data from infants and adults. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 50, 169–179. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.10.009
  • Kobayashi, H., & Kohshima, S. (2001). Unique morphology of the human eye and its adaptive meaning: Comparative studies on external morphology of the primate eye. Journal of Human Evolution, 40(5), 419–435. doi:10.1006/jhev.2001.0468
  • Langton, S. R. (2000). The mutual influence of gaze and head orientation in the analysis of social attention direction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 53(3), 825–845. doi:10.1080/713755908
  • Langton, S. R., Law, A. S., Burton, A. M., & Schweinberger, S. R. (2008). Attention capture by faces. Cognition, 107(1), 330–342. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2007.07.012
  • Langton, S. R., Watt, R. J., & Bruce, V. (2000). Do the eyes have it? Cues to the direction of social attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(2), 50–59. doi:10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01436-9
  • Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31(3), 291–308. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1994.tb02218.x
  • Macrae, C. N., Hood, B. M., Milne, A. B., Rowe, A. C., & Mason, M. F. (2002). Are you looking at me? Eye gaze and person perception. Psychological Science, 13(5), 460–464. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00481
  • Mareschal, I., Calder, A. J., & Clifford, C. W. (2013). Humans have an expectation that gaze is directed toward them. Current Biology, 23(8), 717–721. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.03.030
  • Mareschal, I., Otsuka, Y., & Clifford, C. W. (2014). A generalized tendency toward direct gaze with uncertainty. Journal of Vision, 14(12), doi:10.1167/14.12.27
  • Mormann, F., Niediek, J., Tudusciuc, O., Quesada, C. M., Coenen, V. A., Elger, C. E., & Adolphs, R. (2015). Neurons in the human amygdala encode face identity, but not gaze direction. Nature Neuroscience, 18(11), 1568–1570. doi:10.1038/nn.4139
  • Otsuka, Y., Mareschal, I., Calder, A. J., & Clifford, C. W. (2014). Dual-route model of the effect of head orientation on perceived gaze direction. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(4), 1425–1439. doi:10.1037/a0036151
  • Palanica, A., & Itier, R. J. (2011). Searching for a perceived gaze direction using eye tracking. Journal of Vision, 11(2), 19. doi:10.1167/11.2.19
  • Palanica, A., & Itier, R. J. (2014). Effects of peripheral eccentricity and head orientation on gaze discrimination. Visual Cognition, 22(9–10), 1216–1232. doi:10.1080/13506285.2014.990545
  • Ricciardelli, P., & Driver, J. (2008). Effects of head orientation on gaze perception: How positive congruency effects can be reversed. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(3), 491–504. doi:10.1080/17470210701255457
  • Schreij, D., Owens, C., & Theeuwes, J. (2008). Abrupt onsets capture attention independent of top-down control settings. Perception & Psychophysics, 70(2), 208–218. doi:10.3758/PP.70.2.208
  • Senju, A., Hasegawa, T., & Tojo, Y. (2005). Does perceived direct gaze boost detection in adults and children with and without autism? The stare-in-the-crowd effect revisited. Visual Cognition, 12(8), 1474–1496. doi: 10.1080/13506280444000797
  • Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009a). Atypical eye contact in autism: Models, mechanisms and development. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(8), 1204–1214. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.06.001
  • Senju, A., & Johnson, M. H. (2009b). The eye contact effect: Mechanisms and development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(3), 127–134. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.009
  • Sweeny, T. D., & Whitney, D. (2014). Perceiving crowd attention: Ensemble perception of a crowd's gaze. Psychological Science, 25(10), 1903–1913. doi:10.1177/0956797614544510
  • Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49(1), 83–90. doi:10.3758/Bf03211619
  • Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(6), 599–606. doi:10.3758/BF03211656
  • Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006
  • Towler, J., & Eimer, M. (2015). The focus of spatial attention determines the number and quality of individual faces retained in working memory. Journal of Vision, 15(12), 669. doi:10.1167/15.12.669
  • Vuilleumier, P., George, N., Lister, V., Armony, J., & Driver, J. (2005). Effects of perceived mutual gaze and gender on face processing and recognition memory. Visual Cognition, 12(1), 85–101. doi:10.1080/13506280444000120
  • Yokoyama, T., Sakai, H., Noguchi, Y., & Kita, S. (2014). Perception of direct gaze does not require focus of attention. Scientific Reports, 4, 3858
  • Zhao, M., & Hayward, W. G. (2010). Holistic processing underlies gender judgments of faces. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(3), 591–596. doi:10.3758/APP.72.3.591

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.