381
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Dual Coding of object relations: An alternative framework for understanding perceptual processing in autism?

ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 122-132 | Received 14 Dec 2016, Accepted 12 May 2017, Published online: 16 Jun 2017

References

  • Ahissar, M., & Hochstein, S. (2004). The reverse hierarchy theory of visual perceptual learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 457–464. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.011
  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). The diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM 5. Arlington, VA: bookpointUS.
  • Atkinson, J., & Braddick, O. (2005). Dorsal stream vulnerability and autistic disorders: The importance of comparative studies of form and motion coherence in typically developing children and children with developmental disorders. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive, 23, 49–58.
  • Baylis, G. C., & Driver, J. (1993). Visual attention and objects: Evidence for hierarchical coding of location. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19, 451–470.
  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 258–264. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.05.001
  • Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14(2), 151–157. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.01434
  • Booth, R., Charlton, R., Hughes, C., & Happé, F. (2003). Disentangling weak coherence and executive dysfunction: Planning drawing in autism and attention–deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358, 387–392. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2002.1204
  • Bölte, S., Holtmann, M., Poustka, F., Scheurich, A., & Schmidt, L. (2007). Gestalt perception and local-global processing in high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1493–1504. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0231-x
  • Briskman, J., Frith, U., & Happe, F. (2001). Exploring the cognitive phenotype of autism: Weak “central coherence” in parents and siblings of children with autism: II. Real-life skills and preferences. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 42(03), 309–316. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00724
  • Brosnan, M. J., Scott, F. J., Fox, S., & Pye, J. (2004). Gestalt processing in autism: Failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 459–469. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00237.x
  • Dickinson, A., Bruyns-Haylett, M., Smith, R., Jones, M., & Milne, E. (2016). Superior orientation discrimination and increased peak gamma frequency in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 125(3), 412–422. doi: 10.1037/abn0000148
  • Donnelly, N., Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1991). Parallel computation of primitive shape descriptions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17, 561–570.
  • Duncan, J. (1984). Selective attention and the organization of visual information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 501–517. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.113.4.501
  • Duncan, J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1989). Visual search and stimulus similarity. Psychological Review, 96(3), 433–458. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.433
  • Edgin, J. O., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Spatial cognition in autism spectrum disorders: Superior, impaired, or just intact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(6), 729–745. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0020-y
  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell.
  • Frith, U., & Happé, F. (1994). Autism: Beyond “theory of mind”. Cognition, 50(1), 115–132. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90024-8
  • Han, S., Jiang, Y., Mao, L., Humphreys, G. W., & Qin, J. (2005). Attentional modulation of perceptual grouping in human visual cortex: ERP studies. Human Brain Mapping, 26(3), 199–209. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20157
  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0
  • Happé, F. G. (1997). Central coherence and theory of mind in autism: Reading homographs in context. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15(1), 1–12. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1997.tb00721.x
  • Happé, F. G., & Booth, R. D. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63. doi: 10.1080/17470210701508731
  • Hubl, D., Bölte, S., Feineis–Matthews, S., Lanfermann, H., Federspiel, A., Strik, W., … Dierks, T. (2003). Functional imbalance of visual pathways indicates alternative face processing strategies in autism. Neurology, 61(9), 1232–1237. doi: 10.1212/01.WNL.0000091862.22033.1A
  • Humphreys, G. W. (1998). Neural representation of objects in space: A dual coding account. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 353(1373), 1341–1351. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0288
  • Humphreys, G. W., & Heinke, D. (1998). Spatial representation and selection in the brain: Neuropsychological and computational constraints. Visual Cognition, 5(1–2), 9–47. doi: 10.1080/713756777
  • Humphreys, G. W., Quinlan, P. T., & Riddoch, M. J. (1989). Grouping processes in visual search: Effects with single- and combined-feature targets.. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 258–279. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.258
  • Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1987). To see but not to see: A case study of visual agnosia. Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Humphreys, G. W., & Riddoch, M. J. (1995). Separate coding of space within and between perceptual objects: evidence from unilateral visual neglect. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 12, 283–312. doi: 10.1080/02643299508252000
  • Jones, C. R., Swettenham, J., Charman, T., Marsden, A. J., Tregay, J., Baird, G., … Happé, F. (2011). No evidence for a fundamental visual motion processing deficit in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, 4(5), 347–357. doi: 10.1002/aur.209
  • Lamb, M. R., Robertson, L. C., & Knight, R. T. (1990). Component mechanisms underlying the processing of hierarchically organized patterns: Inferences from patients with unilateral cortical lesions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(3), 471–483.
  • Mann, T. A., & Walker, P. (2003). Autism and a deficit in broadening the spread of visual attention. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(2), 274–284. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00120
  • Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational investigation into the human representation and processing of visual information. New York, NY: Henry Holt and Co. Inc.
  • Martin, E. M., Pitrak, D. L., Robertson, L. C., Novak, R. M., Mullane, K. M., & Pursell, K. J. (1995). Global-local analysis in HIV-1 infection. Neuropsychology, 9(1), 102–109. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.9.1.102
  • Massman, P. J., Delis, D. C., Filoteo, J. V., Butters, N., Salmon, D. P., & Demadura, T. L. (1993). Mechanisms of spatial impairment in alzheimer’s disease subgroups: Differential breakdown of directed attention to global-local stimuli. Neuropsychology, 7(2), 172–181. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.7.2.172
  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 255–263. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00018
  • Mottron, L., Bouvet, L., Bonnel, A., Samson, F., Burack, J. A., Dawson, M., & Heaton, P. (2013). Veridical mapping in the development of exceptional autistic abilities. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 37, 209–228. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.11.016
  • Mottron, L., & Burack, J. (2001). Enhanced perceptual functioning in the development of autism. In J. A. Burack, T. Charman, N. Yirmiya, & P. R. Zelazo (Eds.), The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research (pp. 131–148). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulieres, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7
  • O’Riordan, M. A., Plaisted, K. C., Driver, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2001). Superior visual search in autism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 27, 719–730.
  • Paivio, A. (1990). Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Pellicano, E., & Burr, D. (2012). When the world becomes “too real”: a Bayesian explanation of autistic perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(10), 504–510. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.08.009
  • Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.10.003
  • Pellicano, E., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Maley, A. (2006). Multiple cognitive capabilities/deficits in children with an autism spectrum disorder: “weak” central coherence and its relationship to theory of mind and executive control. Development and Psychopathology, 18, 77–98. doi: 10.1017/S0954579406060056
  • Pirrone, A., Dickinson, A., Gomez, R., Stafford, T., & Milne, E. (2017). Understanding perceptual judgment in autism spectrum disorder using the drift diffusion model. Neuropsychology, 31(2), 173–180. doi: 10.1037/neu0000320
  • Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(5), 733–742. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00489
  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32, 3–25. doi: 10.1080/00335558008248231
  • Riddoch, M. J., & Humphreys, G. W. (1987). A case of integrative visual agnosia. Brain, 110(6), 1431–1462. doi: 10.1093/brain/110.6.1431
  • Riddoch, M. J., Humphreys, G. W., Akhtar, N., Allen, H., Bracewell, R. M., & Schofield, A. J. (2008). A tale of two agnosias: Distinctions between form and integrative agnosia. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 56–92. doi: 10.1080/02643290701848901
  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Tonge, B. J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry, 41, 769–778. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00664
  • Rosenberg, A., Patterson, J. S., & Angelaki, D. E. (2015). A computational perspective on autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(30), 9158–9165. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1510583112
  • Saygin, A. P., Cook, J., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Unaffected perceptual thresholds for biological and non-biological form-from-motion perception in autism spectrum conditions. PloS ONE, 5(10), e13491. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0013491
  • Shafai, F., Armstrong, K., Iarocci, G., & Oruc, I. (2015). Visual orientation processing in autism spectrum disorder: No sign of enhanced early cortical function. Journal of Vision, 15(15), 18. doi: 10.1167/15.15.18
  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1983). An islet of ability in autistic children: A research note. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 24, 613–620. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1983.tb00137.x
  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb02095.x
  • Simmons, D. R., Robertson, A. E., McKay, L. S., Toal, E., McAleer, P., & Pollick, F. E. (2009). Vision in autism spectrum disorders. Vision Research, 49, 2705–2739. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2009.08.005
  • Sinha, P., Kjelgaard, M. M., Gandhi, T. K., Tsourides, K., Cardinaux, A. L., Pantazis, D., … Held, R. M. (2014). Autism as a disorder of prediction. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(42), 15220–15225. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1416797111
  • Smith, A. D., & Gilchrist, I. D. (2005). Within-object and between-object coding deficits in drawing production. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 523–537. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000167
  • Smith, A. D., Kenny, L., Rudnicka, A., Briscoe, J., & Pellicano, E. (2016). Drawing firmer conclusions: Autistic children show no evidence of a local processing bias in a controlled copying task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(11), 3481–3492. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2889-z
  • Sutherland, A., & Crewther, D. P. (2010). Magnocellular visual evoked potential delay with high autism spectrum quotient yields a neural mechanism for altered perception. Brain, 133, 2089–2097. doi: 10.1093/brain/awq122
  • Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: The fourteenth bartlett memorial lecture. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 201–237. doi: 10.1080/02724988843000104
  • Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
  • Van der Hallen, R., Evers, K., Brewaeys, K., Van den Noortgate, W., & Wagemans, J. (2015). Global processing takes time: A meta-analysis on local–global visual processing in ASD. Psychological Bulletin, 141(3), 549–573. doi: 10.1037/bul0000004
  • Wang, L., Mottron, L., Peng, D., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(5), 550–574. doi: 10.1080/13546800701417096
  • White, S. J., & Saldaña, D. (2011). Performance of children with autism on the embedded figures test: A closer look at a popular task. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(11), 1565–1572. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1182-4
  • White, S., O’Reilly, H., & Frith, U. (2009). Big heads, small details and autism. Neuropsychologia, 47(5), 1274–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.012
  • Yerys, B. E., Wallace, G. L., Harrison, B., Celano, M. J., Giedd, J. N., & Kenworthy, L. E. (2009). Set-shifting in children with autism spectrum disorders reversal shifting deficits on the intradimensional/extradimensional shift test correlate with repetitive behaviors. Autism, 13(5), 523–538. doi: 10.1177/1362361309335716

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.