338
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Context-specific distractors rejection: contextual cues control long-term habituation of attentional capture by abrupt onsets

, , &
Pages 291-304 | Received 02 Jul 2018, Accepted 02 Feb 2019, Published online: 22 Feb 2019

References

  • Awh, E., Matsukura, M., & Serences, J. T. (2003). Top-down modulation of biased competition during covert spatial orienting. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(1), 52–63.
  • Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485–496. doi: 10.3758/BF03205306
  • Barry, R. J. (2009). Habituation of the orienting reflex and the development of preliminary process theory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 235–242. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.007
  • Bell, R., Röer, J. P., Dentale, S., & Buchner, A. (2012). Habituation of the irrelevant sound effect: Evidence for an attentional theory of short-term memory disruption. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 38(6), 1542–1557. doi: 10.1037/a0028459
  • Carlisle, N. B., Arita, J. T., Pardo, D., & Woodman, G. F. (2011). Attentional templates in visual working memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(25), 9315–9322. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1097-11.2011
  • Chiandetti, C., & Turatto, M. (2017). Context-specific habituation of the freezing response in newborn chicks. Behavioral Neuroscience, 131(5), 437–446. doi: 10.1037/bne0000212
  • Chun, M. M. (2000). Contextual cueing of visual attention. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4(5), 170–178. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01476-5
  • Chun, M. M., & Jiang, Y. (1998). Contextual cueing: Implicit learning and memory of visual context guides spatial attention. Cognitive Psychology, 36(1), 28–71. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1998.0681
  • Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2013). Context-dependent control over attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 39(3), 836–848. doi: 10.1037/a0030027
  • Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2014). Establishment of an attentional set via statistical learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 1–6. doi: 10.1037/a0034489
  • Cowan, N. (1988). Evolving conceptions of memory storage, selective attention, and their mutual constraints within the human information-processing system. Psychological Bulletin, 104(2), 163–191. Retrieved from http://psycnet.apa.org/journals/bul/104/2/163/
  • Dixon, M. L., Ruppel, J., Pratt, J., & De Rosa, E. (2009). Learning to ignore: Acquisition of sustained attentional suppression. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(2), 418–423. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.418
  • Dukewich, K. R. (2009). Reconceptualizing inhibition of return as habituation of the orienting response. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 238–251. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.238
  • Elliott, E. M., & Cowan, N. (2001). Habituation to auditory distractors in a cross-modal, color-word interference task. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 27(3), 654–667. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.27.3.654
  • Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1153–1165. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000084
  • Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Attentional capture by entirely irrelevant distractors. Visual Cognition, 16(2–3), 200–214. doi: 10.1080/13506280701465049
  • Frey, P. W., & Butler, C. S. (1977). Extinction after aversive conditioning: An associative or nonassociative process? Learning and Motivation, 8(1), 1–17. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(77)90063-7
  • Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2015). Direct evidence for active suppression of salient-but-irrelevant sensory inputs. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1740–1750. doi: 10.1177/0956797615597913
  • Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018a). Distinguishing among potential mechanisms of singleton suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(4), 626–644. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000484
  • Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018b). The role of Inhibition in avoiding distraction by salient stimuli. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22(1), 79–92. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.11.001
  • Gati, I., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1990). Novelty and significance in orientation and habituation: A feature-Matching approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(3), 251–263. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.119.3.251
  • Geng, J. J. (2014). Attentional mechanisms of distractor suppression. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(2), 147–153. doi: 10.1177/0963721414525780
  • Hannula, D. E., & Ranganath, C. (2009). The eyes have it: Hippocampal activity predicts expression of memory in eye movements. Neuron, 63(5), 592–599. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.025
  • Harris, J. D. (1943). Habituatory response decrement in the intact organism. Psychological Bulletin, 40(6), 385–422. doi: 10.1037/h0053918
  • Jonides, J., & Yantis, S. (1988). Uniqueness of abrupt visual onset in capturing attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 43(4), 346–354. doi: 10.3758/BF03208805
  • Jordan, W. P., Strasser, H. C., & McHale, L. (2000). Contextual control of long-term habituation in rats. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 26(3), 323–339. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.26.3.323
  • Kraut, A. G., & Smothergill, D. W. (1978). A two-factor theory of stimulus-Repetition effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 4(1), 191–197.
  • Leber, A. B., Kawahara, J. I., & Gabari, Y. (2009). Long-term abstract learning of attentional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(5), 1385–1397. doi: 10.1037/a0016470
  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
  • Mackintosh, N. J. (1987). Neurobiology, psychology and habituation. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 25(2), 81–97. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(87)90079-9
  • Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 657–672. doi: 10.3758/BF03209251
  • Marini, F., Chelazzi, L., & Maravita, A. (2013). The costly filtering of potential distraction: Evidence for a supramodal mechanism. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142(3), 906–922. doi: 10.1037/a0029905
  • Marlin, N. A., & Miller, R. R. (1981). Associations to contextual stimuli as a determinant of long-term habituation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 7(4), 313–333. doi: 10.1037/0097-7403.7.4.313
  • Müller, H. J., Geyer, T., Zehetleitner, M., & Krummenacher, J. (2009). Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors Is Modulated by top-down dimensional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 1–16. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.1
  • Neo, G., & Chua, F. K. (2006). Capturing focused attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(8), 1286–1296. doi: 10.3758/BF03193728
  • Pascucci, D., & Turatto, M. (2015). The distracting impact of repeated visible and invisible onsets on focused attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 879–892. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000025
  • Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexes. Oxford University Press. (Vol. 17). doi: 10.2307/1134737
  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision, 10, 437–442.
  • Ramaswami, M. (2014). Network plasticity in adaptive filtering and behavioral habituation. Neuron, 82(6), 1216–1229. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.035
  • Rankin, C. H. (2000). Context conditioning in habituation in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Behavioral Neuroscience, 114(3), 496–505. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.114.3.496
  • Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo, J., … Thompson, R. F. (2009). Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 135–138. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012
  • Röer, J. P., Bell, R., & Buchner, A. (2014). Evidence for habituation of the irrelevant-sound effect on serial recall. Memory and Cognition, 42(4), 609–621. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0381-y
  • Ruz, M., & Lupiáñez, J. (2002). A review of attentional capture: On its automaticity and sensitivity to endogenous control. Psicológica, 23, 283–309.
  • Sawaki, R., & Luck, S. J. (2013). Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 20(2), 296–301. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0353-4
  • Schneider, W., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: I. Detection, search, and attention. Psychological Review, 84(1), 1–66. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.1.1
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
  • Sokolov, E. N. (1963). Higher nervous functions: The orienting reflex. Annual Review of Physiology, 25(1), 545–580. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.25.030163.002553
  • Sörqvist, P., Nöstl, A., & Halin, N. (2012). Working memory capacity modulates habituation rate: Evidence from a cross-modal auditory distraction paradigm. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(2), 245–250. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0203-9
  • Sörqvist, P., & Rönnberg, J. (2014). Individual differences in distractibility: An update and a model. PsyCh Journal, 3(1), 42–57. doi: 10.1002/pchj.47
  • Stephenson, D., & Siddle, D. A. T. (1983). Theories of habituation. In D. Siddle (Ed.), Orienting and habituation: Perspectives in human research (pp. 183–236). Chichester: Wiley.
  • Theeuwes, J. (2013). Feature-based attention: It is all bottom-up priming. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1628), 20130055. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0055
  • Thompson, R. F. (2009). Habituation: A history. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.011
  • Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: A model phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior. Psychological Review, 73(1), 16–43. doi: 10.1037/h0022681
  • Tomsic, D., Pedreira, M. E., Romano, A., Hermitte, G., & Maldonado, H. (1998). Context-US association as a determinant of long-term habituation in the crab chasmagnathus. Animal Learning and Behavior, 26(2), 196–209. doi: 10.3758/BF03199212
  • Turatto, M., Bonetti, F., & Pascucci, D. (2018). Filtering visual onsets via habituation: A context-specific long-term memory of irrelevant stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(3), 1028–1034. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1320-x
  • Turatto, M., Bonetti, F., Pascucci, D., & Chelazzi, L. (2018). Desensitizing the attention system to distraction while Idling: A new latent learning phenomenon in the visual attention domain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(12), 1827–1850. doi: 10.1037/xge0000503
  • Turatto, M., & Pascucci, D. (2016). Short-term and long-term plasticity in the visual-attention system: Evidence from habituation of attentional capture. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 130, 159–169. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.02.010
  • Vatterott, D. B., Mozer, M. C., & Vecera, S. P. (2018). Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 80(2), 485–499. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8
  • Vatterott, D. B., & Vecera, S. P. (2012). Experience-dependent attentional tuning of distractor rejection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(5), 871–878. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0280-4
  • Vecera, S. P., Cosman, J. D., Vatterott, D. B., & Roper, Z. J. J. (2014). The control of visual attention: Toward a unified account. Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory, 60. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-800090-8.00008-1
  • Wagner, A. R. (1976). Priming in STM: An information-processing mechanism for self-generated or retrieval-generated depression in performance. In T. J. Tighe, & R. N. Leaton (Eds.), Habituation: Perspectives from child development, animal behavior, and neurophysiology (pp. 95–128). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlabaum.
  • Wagner, A. R. (1978). Expectancies and the priming of STM. In S. H. Hulse, H. Fowler, & R. Honig (Eds.), Cognitive processes in animal behavior (pp. 177–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlabaum. doi: 10.1101/lm.77504.ing
  • Wagner, A. R. (1979). Habituation and memory. In A. Dickinsons, & R. A. Boakes (Eds.), Mechanism of learning and motivation: A memorial volum for Jerzy Konorski (pp. 53–83). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlabaum.
  • Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018). Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture independent of search strategy. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics. doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1562-3
  • Waters, W. F., McDonald, D. G., & Koresko, R. (1977). Habituation of the orienting response: A gating mechanism subserving selective attention. Psychophysiology, 14(3), 228–236.
  • Yantis, S., & Jonides, J. (1984). Abrupt visual onsets and selective attention: Evidence from visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10(5), 601–621. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.10.5.601

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.