419
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Learning to ignore salient distractors: Attentional set and habituation

ORCID Icon &
Pages 214-226 | Received 15 Jul 2018, Accepted 06 Feb 2019, Published online: 22 Feb 2019

References

  • Arita, J. T., Carlisle, N. B., & Woodman, G. F. (2012). Templates for rejection: Configuring attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(3), 580–584. doi: 10.1037/a0027885
  • Awh, E., Belopolsky, A. V., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Top-down versus bottom-up attentional control: A failed theoretical dichotomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(8), 437–443. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.010
  • Bacon, W. F., & Egeth, H. E. (1994). Overriding stimulus-driven attentional capture. Perception & Psychophysics, 55(5), 485–496. doi: 10.3758/BF03205306
  • Baker, T. B., & Tiffany, S. T. (1985). Morphine tolerance as habituation. Psychological Review, 92(1), 78–108. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.92.1.78
  • Barry, R. J. (2009). Habituation of the orienting reflex and the development of preliminary process theory. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92, 235–242. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.007
  • Becker, S. I. (2007). Irrelevant singletons in pop-out search: Attentional capture or filtering costs? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(4), 764–787. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.4.764
  • Becker, M. W., Hemsteger, S., & Peltier, C. (2015). No templates for rejection: A failure to configure attention to ignore task-irrelevant features. Visual Cognition, 23, 1150–1167. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2016.1149532
  • Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., … Rosen, B. R. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17(5), 875–887. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80219-6
  • Cosman, J. D., & Vecera, S. P. (2014). Establishment of an attentional set via statistical learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40(1), 1–6. doi: 10.1037/a0034489
  • Dukewich, K. R. (2009). Reconceptualizing inhibition of return as habituation of the orienting response. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 16(2), 238–251. doi: 10.3758/PBR.16.2.238
  • Folk, C. L., & Remington, R. W. (2015). Unexpected abrupt onsets can override a top-down set for color. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(4), 1153–1165. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000084
  • Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Attentional capture by entirely irrelevant distractors. Visual Cognition, 16(2–3), 200–214. doi: 10.1080/13506280701465049
  • Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2015). Direct evidence for active suppression of salient-but-irrelevant sensory inputs. Psychological Science, 26(11), 1740–1750. doi: 10.1177/0956797615597913
  • Gaspelin, N., Leonard, C. J., & Luck, S. J. (2017). Suppression of overt attentional capture by salient-but-irrelevant color singletons. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 79(1), 45–62. doi: 10.3758/s13414-016-1209-1
  • Gaspelin, N., & Luck, S. J. (2018). Distinguishing among potential mechanisms of singleton suppression. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(4), 626–644. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000484
  • Gati, I., & Ben-Shakhar, G. (1990). Novelty and significance in orientation and habituation: A feature-matching approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 119(3), 251–263. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.119.3.251
  • Graves, T., & Egeth, H. E. (2015). When does feature search fail to protect against attentional capture? Visual Cognition, 23(9–10), 1098–1123. doi: 10.1080/13506285.2016.1145159
  • Groves, P. M., & Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: A dual-process theory. Psychological Review, 77(5), 419–450. doi: 10.1037/h0029810
  • Harris, J. D. (1943). Habituatory response decrement in the intact organism. Psychological Bulletin, 40(6), 385–422. doi: 10.1037/h0053918
  • Jonides, J. (1981). Voluntary versus automatic control over the mind’s eye’s movements. In J. B. Baddeley, & A. D. Long (Eds.), Attention and performance IX (pp. 187–203). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Konorski, J. (1967). Integrative activity of the brain; an interdisciplinary approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
  • Koukounas, E., & Over, R. (2001). Habituation of male sexual arousal: Effects of attentional focus. Biological Psychology, 58(1), 49–64. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(01)00096-5
  • Laan, E., & Everaerd, W. (1995). Habituation of female sexual arousal to slides and film. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 24(5), 517–541. doi: 10.1007/BF01541832
  • Lamy, D., & Egeth, H. E. (2003). Attentional capture in singleton-detection and feature-search modes. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1003–1020. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1003
  • Leber, A. B., & Egeth, H. E. (2006). Attention on autopilot: Past experience and attentional set. Visual Cognition, 14(4–8), 565–583. doi: 10.1080/13506280500193438
  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
  • Müller, H. J., Geyer, T., Zehetleitner, M., & Krummenacher, J. (2009). Attentional capture by salient color singleton distractors is modulated by top-down dimensional set. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(1), 1–16. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.35.1.1
  • Neo, G., & Chua, F. K. (2006). Capturing focused attention. Perception & Psychophysics, 68(8), 1286–1296. doi: 10.3758/BF03193728
  • Pascucci, D., & Turatto, M. (2015). The distracting impact of repeated visible and invisible onsets on focused attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 41(3), 879–892. doi:10.1037/xhp0000025
  • Pavlov, I. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Pelli, D. G. (1997). The VideoToolbox software for visual psychophysics: Transforming numbers into movies. Spatial Vision. doi: 10.1163/156856897X00366
  • Rankin, C. H., Abrams, T., Barry, R. J., Bhatnagar, S., Clayton, D. F., Colombo, J., … Thompson, R. F. (2009). Habituation revisited: An updated and revised description of the behavioral characteristics of habituation. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 135–138. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.09.012
  • Shiffrin, R. M., & Schneider, W. (1977). Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory. Psychological Review, 84(2), 127–190. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.127
  • Sokolov, E. N. (1960). Neural model and the orienting influence. In M. A. Brazier (Ed.), The central nervous system and behavior (pp. 187–271). New York: Macy Foundation.
  • Sokolov, E. N. (1963, September). Higher nervous functions: The orienting reflex. Annual Review of Physiology, 25(1), 545–580. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ph.25.030163.002553
  • Sörqvist, P. (2010). High working memory capacity attenuates the deviation effect but not the changing-state effect: Further support for the duplex-mechanism account of auditory distraction. Memory and Cognition, 38(5), 651–658. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.5.651
  • Sörqvist, P., Dahlström, Ö, Karlsson, T., & Rönnberg, J. (2016, May). Attachment styles among \bung adults: A test of a four-category model. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00221
  • Sörqvist, P., & Rönnberg, J. (2014). Individual differences in distractibility: An update and a model. PsyCh Journal, 3, 42–57. doi:10.1002/pchj.47
  • Steiner, G. Z., & Barry, R. J. (2014). The mechanism of dishabituation. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 8, 1–8. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00014
  • Theeuwes, J. (2004). Top-down search strategies cannot override attentional capture. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 11(1), 65–70. doi: 10.3758/BF03206462
  • Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006
  • Thompson, R. F. (2009). Habituation: A history. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 92(2), 127–134. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2008.07.011
  • Turatto, M., Bonetti, F., & Pascucci, D. (2018). Filtering visual onsets via habituation: A context-specific long-term memory of irrelevant stimuli. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25(3), 1028–1034. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1320-x
  • Turatto, M., Bonetti, F., Pascucci, D., & Chelazzi, L. (2018). Desensitizing the attention system to distraction while idling: A new latent learning phenomenon in the visual attention domain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147, 1827–1850. doi: 10.1037/xge0000503
  • Turatto, M., & Pascucci, D. (2016). Short-term and long-term plasticity in the visual-attention system: Evidence from habituation of attentional capture. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 130, 159–169. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.02.010
  • Vatterott, D. B., Mozer, M. C., & Vecera, S. P. (2018). Rejecting salient distractors: Generalization from experience. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 80(2), 485–499. doi: 10.3758/s13414-017-1465-8
  • Vatterott, D. B., & Vecera, S. P. (2012). Experience-dependent attentional tuning of distractor rejection. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 19(5), 871–878. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0280-4
  • Wagner, A. R. (1979). Habituation and memory. In A. Dickinson, & R. A. Boakes (Eds.), Mechanisms of learning and motivation: A memorial volume for Jerry Konorski (pp. 53–82). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum Assoc.
  • Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018a). Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 44(1), 13–17. doi: 10.1037/xhp0000472
  • Wang, B., & Theeuwes, J. (2018b). Statistical regularities modulate attentional capture independent of search strategy. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 44(1), 13–17. doi: 10.3758/s13414-018-1562-3
  • Waters, W. F., McDonald, D. G., & Koresko, R. L. (1977). Habituation of the orienting response: A gating mechanism subserving selective attention. Psychophysiology. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1977.tb01166.x
  • Woodman, G. F., & Luck, S. J. (2007). Do the contents of visual working memory automatically influence attentional Selection during visual search? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33(2), 363–377. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.33.2.363
  • Zehetleitner, M., Goschy, H., & Müller, H. J. (2012). Top-down control of attention: It’s gradual, practice-dependent, and hierarchically organized. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38(4), 941–957. doi: 10.1037/a0027629

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.