287
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Forthcoming Special Issue on: Visual Search and Selective Attention

Attentional priming does not enable observers to ignore salient distractors

ORCID Icon &
Pages 595-608 | Received 28 Feb 2019, Accepted 13 Aug 2019, Published online: 22 Aug 2019

References

  • Ásgeirsson, Á. G., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2011). Episodic retrieval and feature facilitation in intertrial priming of visual search. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 73, 1350–1360. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0119-5
  • Ásgeirsson, Á. G., Kristjánsson, Á., & Bundesen, C. (2014). Independent priming of location and color in identification of briefly presented letters. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 76, 40–48. doi: 10.3758/s13414-013-0546-6
  • Ásgeirsson, Á. G., Kristjánsson, Á., & Bundesen, C. (2015). Repetition priming in selective attention: A TVA analysis. Acta Psychologica, 160, 35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.06.008
  • Baluch, F., & Itti, L. (2011). Mechanisms of top-down attention. Trends in Neurosciences, 34(4), 210–224. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.02.003
  • Belopolsky, A. V., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2010). What is top-down about contingent capture? Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(2), 326–341. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.2.326
  • Brascamp, J. W., Pels, E., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2011). Priming of pop-out on multiple time scales during visual search. Vision Research, 51(17), 1972–1978. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2011.07.007
  • Bundesen, C. (1990). A theory of visual attention. Psychological Review, 97, 523–547. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.97.4.523
  • Crump, M. J. C., Mcdonnell, J. V., & Gureckis, T. M. (2013). Evaluating Amazon’s mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. Plos One, 8, 3. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0057410
  • Desimone, R., & Duncan, J. S. (1995). Neural mechanisms of selective visual attention. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 18, 193–222. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.001205
  • Folk, C. L., Remington, R. W., & Johnston, J. C. (1992). Involuntary covert orienting is contingent on attentional control settings. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18(4), 1030–1044. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.18.4.1030
  • Forster, S., & Lavie, N. (2008). Failures to ignore entirely irrelevant distractors: The role of load. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14(1), 73–83. doi: 10.1037/1076-898X.14.1.73
  • Germine, L., Nakayama, K., Duchaine, B. C., & Wilmer, J. B. (2012). Is the web as good as the lab? Comparable performance from Web and lab in cognitive / perceptual experiments. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 847–857. doi: 10.3758/s13423-012-0296-9
  • Goolsby, B. A., & Suzuki, S. (2001). Understanding priming of color-singleton search: Roles of attention at encoding and “retrieval”. Perception and Psychophysics, 63(6), 929–944. doi: 10.3758/BF03194513
  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding (TEC): A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/12239891 doi: 10.1017/S0140525X01000103
  • Huang, L., Holcombe, A. O., & Pashler, H. (2004). Repetition priming in visual search: Episodic retrieval, not feature priming. Memory & Cognition, 32(1), 12–20. doi: 10.3758/BF03195816
  • Kleiner, M., Brainard, D., & Pelli, D. (2007). What’s new in psychtoolbox-3? Perception, 36, ECVP Abstract Supplement.
  • Kristjánsson, Á., & Ásgeirsson, Á. G. (2019). Attentional priming: Recent insights and current controversies. Current Opinion in Psychology, 29, 71–75. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.11.013
  • Kristjánsson, Á, & Campana, G. (2010). Where perception meets memory: A review of repetition priming in visual search tasks. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 72(1), 5–18. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.1.5
  • Kristjánsson, A., Wang, D., & Nakayama, K. (2002). The role of priming in conjunctive visual search. Cognition, 85(1), 37–52. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00074-4
  • Lamy, D. F., & Kristjánsson, Á. (2013). Is goal-directed attentional guidance just intertrial priming? A review. Journal of Vision, 13, 14–14. doi: 10.1167/13.3.14
  • Liesefeld, H. R., Liesefeld, A. M., & Müller, H. J. (2019). Distractor-interference reduction is dimensionally constrained. Visual Cognition, doi: 10.1080/13506285.2018.1561568
  • Maljkovic, V., & Nakayama, K. (1994). Priming of pop-out: I. Role of features. Memory & Cognition, 22(6), 657–672. doi: 10.3758/BF03209251
  • Martini, P. (2010). System identification in priming of pop-out. Vision Research, 50(21), 2110–2115. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2010.07.024
  • Mathôt, S., Schreij, D., & Theeuwes, J. (2012). Opensesame: An open-source, graphical experiment builder for the social sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 44(2), 314–324. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0168-7
  • Morey, R. D. (2008). Confidence intervals from normalized data: A correction to Cousineau (2005). Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 4(2), 61–64. doi: 10.20982/tqmp.04.2.p061
  • Nordfang, M., & Bundesen, C. (2010). Is initial visual selection completely stimulus-driven? Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 106–108. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.04.013
  • Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49(1), 83–90. doi: 10.3758/BF03211619
  • Theeuwes, J. (1992). Perceptual selectivity for color and form. Perception & Psychophysics, 51(6), 599–606. doi: 10.3758/BF03211656
  • Theeuwes, J. (2010). Top-down and bottom-up control of visual selection. Acta Psychologica, 135(2), 77–99. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.006
  • Theeuwes, J. (2013). Feature-based attention: It is all bottom-up priming. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1628), 20130055. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0055
  • Theeuwes, J., De Vries, G. J., & Godijn, R. (2003). Attentional and oculomotor capture with static singletons. Perception & Psychophysics, 65(5), 735–746. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2012.07.024 doi: 10.3758/BF03194810
  • Theeuwes, J., Reimann, B., & Mortier, K. (2006). Visual search for featural singletons: No top-down modulation, only bottom-up priming. Visual Cognition, 14(4-8), 466–489. doi: 10.1080/13506280500195110
  • Theeuwes, J., & van der Burg, E. (2011). On the limits of top-down control of visual selection. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 73(7), 2092–2103. doi: 10.3758/s13414-011-0176-9
  • Wang, D., Kristjansson, A., & Nakayama, K. (2005). Efficient visual search without top-down or bottom-up guidance. Perception & Psychophysics, 67(2), 239–253. doi: 10.3758/BF03206488
  • Wolfe, J. M. (1994). Guided search 2.0 A revised model of visual search. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 1(2), 202–238. doi: 10.3758/BF03200774
  • Wolfe, J. M., Cave, K. R., & Franzel, S. L. (1989). Guided search: An alternative to the feature integration model for visual search. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15(3), 419–433. doi: 2527952
  • Zhang, B., Allenmark, F., Liesefeld, H. R., Shi, Z., & Müller, H. J. (2019). Probability cueing of singleton-distractor locations in visual search: Priority-map- vs. dimension-based inhibition? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, Article in press; preprint available on bioRxiv. doi: 10.1101/454140

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.