350
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Commentary

Probabilities, causation, and logic programming in conditional reasoning: reply to Stenning and Van Lambalgen (2016)

&
Pages 336-354 | Received 30 Jun 2015, Accepted 05 Jan 2016, Published online: 08 Mar 2016

References

  • Adams, E. W. (1998). A primer of probability logic. Stanford: CLSI.
  • Ali, N., Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (2011). The mental representation of causal conditional inference: Causal models or mental models. Cognition, 119, 403–418. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.02.005
  • Ali, N., Schlottmann, A., Shaw, C., Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (2010). Conditionals and causal discounting in children. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), Cognition and conditionals: Probability and logic in human thinking (pp. 117–134). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Baggio, G., Choma, T., van Lambalgen, M., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Coercion and compositionality: An ERP study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 2131–2140. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21303
  • Baratgin, J., Over, D. E., & Politzer, G. (2013). Uncertainty and the de Finetti tables. Thinking & Reasoning, 19, 308–328. doi:10.1080/13546783.2013.809018
  • Bovens, L., & Hartmann, S. (2003). Bayesian epistemology. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Byrne, R. M. (1989). Suppressing valid inferences with conditionals. Cognition, 31, 61–83. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(89)90018-8
  • Cartwright, N. (1983). How the laws of physics lie. Oxford: Clarendon Press.
  • Chater, N., Goodman, N., Griffiths, T., Kemp, C., Oaksford, M., & Tenenbaum, J. (2011). The imaginary fundamentalists: The unshocking truth about Bayesian cognitive science. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 34, 194–196. doi:10.1017/S0140525X11000239
  • Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (2006). Mental mechanisms. In K. Fiedler & P. Juslin (Eds.), Information sampling and adaptive cognition (pp. 210–236). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Chater, N., & Oaksford, M. (Eds.) (2008). The probabilistic mind: Prospects for Bayesian cognitive science. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Cheng, P. W. (1997). From covariation to causation: A causal power theory. Psychological Review, 104, 367–405. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.104.2.367
  • Chisolm, R. (1946). The contrary-to-fact conditional. Mind, 55, 289–307. doi: 10.1093/mind/LV.219.289
  • Cohen, J. D., & Servan-Schreiber, D. (1992). Context, cortex, and dopamine: A connectionist approach to behavior and biology in schizophrenia. Psychological Review, 99, 45–77. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.99.1.45
  • Cruz, N., Baratgin, J., Oaksford, M., & Over, D. E. (2015). Reasoning with if and ands and ors. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 192. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00192
  • Cummins, D. D. (1995). Naïve theories and causal deduction. Memory & Cognition, 23, 646–658. doi:10.3758/BF03197265
  • Douven, I., & Verbrugge, S. (2013). The probabilities of conditionals revisited. Cognitive Science, 37, 711–730. doi:10.1111/cogs.12025
  • Edgington, D. (1995). On conditionals. Mind, 104, 235–329. doi:10.1093/mind/104.414.235
  • Evans, J. St. B. T. (2009). In two minds: Dual processes and beyond. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Fernbach, P. M., & Erb, C. D. (2013). A quantitative causal model theory of conditional reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1327–1343. doi:10.1037/a0031851
  • Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Fodor, J. A. (2001). The mind doesn't work that way. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Fugard, J. B., Pfeifer, N., Mayerhofer, B., & Kleiter, G. (2011). How people interpret conditionals: Shifts toward the conditional event. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 37, 635–648. doi:10.1037/a0022329
  • Geiger, S. M., & Oberauer, K. (2007). Reasoning with conditionals: Does every counterexample count? It's frequency that counts. Memory & Cognition, 35, 2060–2074. doi:10.3758/BF03192938
  • Gilio, A., & Over, D. (2012). The psychology of inferring conditionals from disjunctions: A probabilistic study. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 56, 118–131. doi:10.1016/j.jmp.2012.02.006
  • Goodman, N. (1947). The problem of counterfactual conditionals. Journal of Philosophy, 44, 113–128. doi:10.2307/2019988
  • Goodman, N. (1955). Fact, fiction, and forecast. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Griffiths, T. L., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2005). Structure and strength in causal induction. Cognitive Psychology, 51(4), 334–384. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2005.05.004
  • Haack, S. (1974). Deviant logic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Hahn, U. (2014). The Bayesian boom: Good thing or bad? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 765. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00765
  • Hahn, U., Harris, A. J. L., & Corner, A. J. (2009). Argument content and argument source: An exploration. Informal Logic, 29, 337–367.
  • Hahn, U., & Oaksford, M. (2007). The rationality of informal argumentation: A Bayesian approach to reasoning fallacies. Psychological Review, 114, 704–732. doi: 10.1037/0033-295x.114.3.704
  • Hahn, U., Oaksford, M., & Harris, A. J. L. (2013). Testimony and argument: A Bayesian perspective. In F. Zenker (Ed.), Bayesian argumentation (pp. 15–38). Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Harris, A. L., & Hahn, U. (2009). Bayesian rationality in evaluating multiple testimonies: Incorporating the role of coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 1366–1373. doi:10.1037/a0016567
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Kowalski, R. (2010). Reasoning with conditionals in artificial intelligence. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), Cognition and conditionals: Probability and logic in human thinking (pp. 254–282). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Kowalski, R., & Sergot, M. (1986). A logic-based calculus of events. New Generation Computing, 4, 67–94. ISSN: 0288-3635
  • Lake, C., Ziegler, M. G., & Murphy, D. L. (1977). Increased norepinephrine levels and decreased dopamine-β-hydroxylase activity in primary autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 34, 553–556. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1977.01770170063005
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press
  • Lakoff, G., & Johnson, M. (1999). Philosophy in the flesh: The embodied mind and its challenge to Western thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Lewis, D. (1973). Counterfactuals. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Manktelow, K. I. (2012). Thinking and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Martignon, L., Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (in preparation). Qualitative models of reasoning and judgment: Theoretical analysis and experimental exploration.
  • Mercier, H., & Sperber, D. (2011). Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 57–74. doi:10.1017/S0140525×10000968
  • Nute, D. (1984). Conditional logic. In D. Gabbay and F. Guenthner (Eds.), Handbook of philosophical logic (Vol. 11, pp. 387–439). Dordrecht: D. Reidel.
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (1994). A rational analysis of the selection task as optimal data selection. Psychological Review, 101(4), 608–631. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.4.608
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (Eds.). (1998). Rational models of cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2007). Bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2010a). Causation and conditionals in the cognitive science of human reasoning. Open Psychology Journal, 3, 105–118. (Special issue in J. C. Perales & D. R. Shanks (Eds.), Causal learning beyond causal judgment). doi: 10.2174/1874350101003010105
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2010b). Conditionals and constraint satisfaction: Reconciling mental models and the probabilistic approach? In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), Cognition and conditionals: Probability and logic in human thinking (pp. 309–334). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2013). Dynamic inference and everyday conditional reasoning in the new paradigm. Thinking and Reasoning, 19, 346–379. doi:10.1080/13546783.2013.808163
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (2014). Probabilistic single function dual process theory and logic programming as approaches to non-monotonicity in human vs. artificial reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 20, 269–295. doi:10.1080/13546783.2013.877401
  • Oaksford, M., & Chater, N. (in press). Causal models and conditional reasoning. In M. Waldmann (Ed.), Oxford handbook of causal cognition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Oaksford, M., & Hahn, U. (2013). Why are we convinced by the ad hominem argument?: Bayesian source reliability and pragma-dialectical discussion rules. In F. Zenker (Ed.), Bayesian argumentation (pp. 39–59). Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Over, D. E. (2009). New paradigm psychology of reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 15, 431–438. doi: 10.1080/13546780903266188
  • Over, D. E., Hadjichristidis, C., Evans, J. St. B. T., Handley, S. J., & Sloman, S. A. (2007). The probability of causal conditionals. Cognitive Psychology, 54, 62–97. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2006.05.002
  • Pearl, J. (1988). Probabilistic reasoning in intelligent systems: Networks of plausible inference. San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
  • Pearl, J. (2000). Causality: Models, reasoning, and inference. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Pfeifer, N., & Kleiter, G. (2010). Mental probability logic. In M. Oaksford & N. Chater (Eds.), Cognition and conditionals: Probability and logic in human thinking (pp. 153–173). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., & Hagoort, P. (2010). Reasoning with exceptions: An event-related brain potentials study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 471–480. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21360
  • Pijnacker, J., Geurts, B., van Lambalgen, M., Buitelaar, J., Kan, C., & Hagoort, P. (2009). Conditional reasoning in high-functioning adults with autism: Evidence for impaired exception-handling. Neuropsychologia, 47, 644–651. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.11.011
  • Politzer, G. (2005). Uncertainty and the suppression of inferences. Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 5–33. doi: 10.1080/13546780442000088
  • Poole, D. (1993). Probabilistic Horn abduction and Bayesian networks. Artificial Intelligence, 64, 81–129. doi:10.1016/0004-3702(93)90061-F
  • Rehder, B. (2014). Independence and dependence in human causal reasoning. Cognitive Psychology, 72, 54–107. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2014.02.002
  • Rumelhart, D. E., Smolensky, P., McClelland, J. L., & Hinton, G. E. (1988). Schemata and sequential thought processes in PDP models. In A. M. Collins, & E. E. Smith (Eds.), Readings in cognitive science: A perspective from psychology and artificial intelligence (pp. 224–249). San Mateo, CA: Morgan Kaufmann.
  • Sellen, J., Oaksford, M., & Gray, N. (2005). Schizotypy and conditional inference. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 31, 105–116. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbi012
  • Shanahan, M. (2009). The frame problem. Retrieved December 12, 2015, from http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/frame-problem/
  • Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 3–22. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.119.1.3
  • Sloman, S., Barbey, A. K., & Hotaling, J. M. (2009). A causal model theory of the meaning of cause, enable, and prevent. Cognitive Science, 33, 21–50. doi:10.1111/j.1551-6709.2008.01002.x
  • Sowa, J. F. (2000). Processes and causality. Retrieved September 30, 2015, from http://www.jfsowa.com/ontology/causal.htm
  • Stalnaker, R. C. (1968). A theory of conditionals. In N. Rescher (Ed.), Studies in logical theory (American Philosophical Quarterly Monograph Series, 2) (pp. 98–112). Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Stalnaker, R. C. (1984). Inquiry. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Stanovich, K. E. (2011). Rationality and the reflective mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Stenning, K., & van Lambalgen, M. (2005). Semantic interpretation as computation in nonmonotonic logic: The real meaning of the suppression task. Cognitive Science, 29, 919–960. doi:10.1207/s15516709cog0000_36
  • Stenning, K., & Van Lambalgen, M. (2016). Logic programming, probability, and two-system accounts of reasoning: A rejoinder to Oaksford and Chater. Thinking and Reasoning. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1080/13546783.2016.1139504
  • Sternberg, D. A., & McClelland, J. L. (2012). Two mechanisms of human contingency learning. Psychological Science, 23(1), 59–68. doi: 10.1177/0956797611429577
  • Thomas, M. S. C., & Mareschal, D. (2001). Metaphor as categorisation: A connectionist implementation. In J. A. Barnden & M. G. Lee (Eds.), Metaphor and artificial intelligence. Metaphor & Symbol 16 (pp. 5–27). London: Psychology Press.
  • van Lambalgen, M., & Hamm, F. (2004). The proper treatment of events. Oxford: Blackwell.
  • Wernhard, C. (2011). Forward human reasoning modelled by logic programming modelled by classical logic with circumscription and projection (Technical Report Knowledge Representation and Reasoning 11-07). Technische Universität Dresden. Retrieved from http://www.wv.inf.tu-dresden.de/Publications/2011/report11-07.pdf

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.