1,623
Views
7
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Empirical Paper

Developmental grey matter changes in superior parietal cortex accompany improved transitive reasoning

, , , , , & show all
Pages 151-170 | Received 08 May 2017, Accepted 21 May 2018, Published online: 03 Oct 2018

References

  • Acuna, B. D., Eliassen, J. C., Donoghue, J. P., & Sanes, J. N. (2002). Frontal and parietal lobe activation during transitive inference in humans. Cerebral Cortex, 12(12), 1312–1321. doi:10.1093/cercor/12.12.1312
  • Amorapanth, P. X., Widick, P., & Chatterjee, A. (2010). The neural basis for spatial relations. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(8), 1739–1753. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21322
  • Andrews, G., & Halford, G. S. (1998). Children's ability to make transitive inferences: The importance of premise integration and structural complexity. Cognitive Development, 13(4), 479–513. doi:10.1016/s0885-2014(98)90004-1
  • Andrews, G., & Halford, G. S. (2002). A cognitive complexity metric applied to cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 45(2), 153–219. doi:10.1016/s0010-0285(02)00002-6
  • Ashburner, J., & Friston, K. J. (2000). Voxel-based morphometry: The methods. Neuroimage, 11(6), 805–821. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0582
  • Bermudez, J. L. (2002). Rationality and psychological explanation without language. In J. L. Bermudez & A. Millar (Eds.), Reason and nature: Essays in the theory of rationality (pp. 233–264). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Blakemore, S. J., & Choudhury, S. (2006). Brain development during puberty: State of the science. Developmental Science, 9(1), 11–14. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2005.00456.x
  • Brett, M., Anton J., Valabregue, R., & Poline, J. (2002, June). Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox. Paper presented at the 8th International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, Sendai, Japan.
  • Brunamonti, E., Genovesio, A., Carbe, K., & Ferraina, S. (2011). Gaze modulates non-propositional reasoning: Further evidence for spatial representation of reasoning premises. Neuroscience, 173, 110–115. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.11.011
  • Bryant, P. E., & Trabasso, T. (1971). Transitive inferences and memory in young children. Nature, 232(5311), 456–458. doi:10.1038/232456a0
  • Cohen, M. S., Kosslyn, S. M., Breiter, H. C., DiGirolamo, G. J., Thompson, W. L., Anderson, A. K., … Belliveau, J. W. (1996). Changes in cortical activity during mental rotation: A mapping study using functional MRI. Brain, 119, 89–100. doi:10.1093/brain/119.1.89
  • Crone, E. A., Wendelken, C., van Leijenhorst, L., Honomichl, R. D., Christoff, K., & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Neurocognitive development of relational reasoning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 55–66. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00743.x
  • Delius, J. D., & Siemann, M. (1998). Transitive responding in animals and humans: Exaptation rather than adaptation? Behavioural Processes, 42(2–3), 107–137. doi:10.1016/s0376-6357(97)00072-7
  • Dempster, A. P., Laird, N. M., & Rubin, D. B. (1977). Maximum likelihood from incomplete data via em algorithm. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society Series B-Methodological, 39(1), 1–38. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/2984875
  • Dumontheil, I., Houlton, R., Christoff, K., & Blakemore, S. J. (2010). Development of relational reasoning during adolescence. Developmental Science, 13(6), F15–F24. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01014.x
  • Eslinger, P. J., Blair, C., Wang, J. L., Lipovsky, B., Realmuto, J., Baker, D., … Yang, Q. X. (2009). Developmental shifts in fMRI activations during visuospatial relational reasoning. Brain and Cognition, 69(1), 1–10. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.04.010
  • Fangmeier, T., Knauff, M., Ruff, C. C., & Sloutsky, V. M. (2006). fMRI evidence for a three-stage model of deductive reasoning. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18(3), 320–334. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.3.320
  • Ferrer, E., O'Hare, E. D., & Bunge, S. A. (2009). Fluid reasoning and the developing brain. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3(1), 46–51. doi:10.3389/neuro.01.003.2009
  • Gazes, R. P., Lazareva, O. F., Bergene, C. N., & Hampton, R. R. (2014). Effects of spatial training on transitive inference performance in humans and rhesus monkeys. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Animal Learning and Cognition, 40(4), 477–489. doi:10.1037/xan0000038
  • Giedd, J. N. (2008). The teen brain: Insights from neuroimaging. Journal of Adolescent Health, 42(4), 335–343. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2008.01.007
  • Goel, V. (2007). Anatomy of deductive reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 435–441. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2007.09.003
  • Goel, V. (2015). Indeterminacy tolerance as a basis of hemispheric asymmetry within prefrontal cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 326–326. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00326
  • Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Functional neuroanatomy of three-term relational reasoning. Neuropsychologia, 39(9), 901–909. doi:10.1016/s0028-3932(01)00024-0
  • Goel, V., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Reciprocal neural response within lateral and ventral medial prefrontal cortex during hot and cold reasoning. Neuroimage, 20(4), 2314–2321. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.07.027
  • Goel, V., Makale, M., & Grafman, J. (2004). The hippocampal system mediates logical reasoning about familiar spatial environments. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16(4), 654–664. doi:10.1162/089892904323057362
  • Goel, V., Navarrete, G., Noveck, I. A., & Prado, J. (2016). Editorial: The reasoning brain: The interplay between cognitive neuroscience and theories of reasoning. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10, 673. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00673
  • Goel, V., Stollstorff, M., Nakic, M., Knutson, K., & Grafman, J. (2009). A role for right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex in reasoning about indeterminate relations. Neuropsychologia, 47(13), 2790–2797. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.06.002
  • Goel, V., Tierney, M., Sheesley, L., Bartolo, A., Vartanian, O., & Grafman, J. (2007). Hemispheric specialization in human prefrontal cortex for resolving certain and uncertain inferences. Cerebral Cortex, 17(10), 2245–2250. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl132
  • Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D., Vaituzis, A. C., … Thompson, P. M. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human cortical development during childhood through early adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101(21), 8174–8179. doi:10.1073/pnas.0402680101
  • Goodwin, G. P., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2005). Reasoning about relations. Psychological Review, 112(2), 468–493. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.112.2.468
  • Hinton, E. C., Dymond, S., von Hecker, U., & Evans, C. J. (2010). Neural correlates of relational reasoning and the symbolic distance effect: involvement of parietal cortex. Neuroscience, 168(1), 138–148. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.03.052
  • Husain, M., & Nachev, P. (2007). Space and the parietal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(1), 30–36. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.011
  • Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1986). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Kallio, K. D. (1982). Developmental-change on a 5-term transitive inference. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 33(1), 142–164. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(82)90011-x
  • Knauff, M., Fangmeier, T., Ruff, C. C., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2003). Reasoning, models, and images: Behavioral measures and cortical activity. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(4), 559–573. doi:10.1162/089892903321662949
  • Lenroot, R. K., & Giedd, J. N. (2006). Brain development in children and adolescents: Insights from anatomical magnetic resonance imaging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(6), 718–729. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.001
  • Maldjian, J. A., Laurienti, P. J., Kraft, R. A., & Burdette, J. H. (2003). An automated method for neuroanatomic and cytoarchitectonic atlas-based interrogation of fMRI data sets. Neuroimage, 19(3), 1233–1239. doi:10.1016/s1053-8119(03)00169-1
  • Mani, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1982). The mental representation of spatial descriptions. Memory & Cognition, 10(2), 181–187. doi:10.3758/bf03209220
  • Markovits, H., & Dumas, C. (1999). Developmental patterns in the understanding of social and physical transitivity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 73(2), 95–114. doi:10.1006/jecp.1999.2496
  • Mims, M., Cantor, J. H., & Riley, C. A. (1983). The development of representation skills in transitive reasoning based on relations of equality and inequality. Child Development, 54(6), 1457–1469. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1983.tb00061.x
  • Moses, S. N., Villate, C., Binns, M. A., Davidson, P. S. R., & Ryan, J. D. (2008). Cognitive integrity predicts transitive inference performance bias and success. Neuropsychologia, 46(5), 1314–1325. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.12.009
  • Petersen, A. C., Crockett, L., Richards, M., & Boxer, A. (1988). A self-report measure of pubertal status - reliability, validity, and initial norms. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 17(2), 117–133. doi:10.1007/bf01537962
  • Piaget, J. (1921). Une forme verbal de la comparison chez l’enfant. Archives de Psychologie, 18, 141–172.
  • Prado, J., Chadha, A., & Booth, J. R. (2011). The brain network for deductive reasoning: A quantitative meta-analysis of 28 neuroimaging studies. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(11), 3483–3497.
  • Prado, J., Van Der Henst, J.-B., & Noveck, I. A. (2010). Recomposing a fragmented literature: How conditional and relational arguments engage different neural systems for deductive reasoning. Neuroimage, 51(3), 1213–1221. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.026
  • Ryan, J. D., Moses, S. N., & Villate, C. (2009). Impaired relational organization of propositions, but intact transitive inference, in aging: Implications for understanding underlying neural integrity. Neuropsychologia, 47(2), 338–353. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.09.006
  • Sack, A. T. (2009). Parietal cortex and spatial cognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 202(2), 153–161. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2009.03.012
  • Shaw, P., Kabani, N. J., Lerch, J. P., Eckstrand, K., Lenroot, R., Gogtay, N., … Wise, S. P. (2008). Neurodevelopmental trajectories of the human cerebral cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(14), 3586–3594. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.5309-07.2008
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual differences in reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645-+. doi:10.1017/s0140525x00003435
  • Sternberg, R. J. (1980). Development of linear syllogistic reasoning. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 29(2), 340–356. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(80)90025-9
  • Stiles, J., & Jernigan, T. L. (2010). The basics of brain development. Neuropsychology Review, 20(4), 327–348. doi:10.1007/s11065-010-9148-4
  • Stollstorff, M., Vartanian, O., & Goel, V. (2012). Levels of conflict in reasoning modulate right lateral prefrontal cortex. Brain Research, 1428, 24–32. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.05.045
  • TEA, S.A. (1999). WAIS-III: Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para Adultos. Tercera versión [WAIS-III: Wechsler adult Intelligence scale. Third version]. Madrid, Spain: TEA.
  • Vartanian, O., Goel, V., Tierney, M., Huey, E. D., & Grafman, J. (2009). Frontotemporal dementia selectively impairs transitive reasoning about familiar spatial environments. Neuropsychology, 23(5), 619–626. doi:10.1037/a0015810
  • Vendetti, M. S., & Bunge, S. A. (2014). Evolutionary and developmental changes in the lateral frontoparietal network: A little goes a long way for higher-level cognition. Neuron, 84(5), 906–917. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.035
  • Waechter, R. L., Goel, V., Raymont, V., Kruger, F., & Grafman, J. (2013). Transitive inference reasoning is impaired by focal lesions in parietal cortex rather than rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychologia, 51(3), 464–471. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.026
  • Waltz, J. A., Knowlton, B. J., Holyoak, K. J., Boone, K. B., Mishkin, F. S., Santos, M. D., … Miller, B. L. (1999). A system for relational reasoning in human prefrontal cortex. Psychological Science, 10(2), 119–125. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00118
  • Wechsler, D. (2005). WISC-IV, Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-IV. Spanish version. [Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—Fourth Edition]. Madrid, Spain: TEA.
  • Wendelken, C. (2015). Meta-analysis: How does posterior parietal cortex contribute to reasoning? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8 1042. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.01042
  • Wendelken, C., & Bunge, S. A. (2010). Transitive inference: Distinct contributions of rostrolateral prefrontal cortex and the hippocampus. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 837–847. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21226
  • Wendelken, C., O'Hare, E. D., Whitaker, K. J., Ferrer, E., & Bunge, S. A. (2011). Increased functional selectivity over development in rostrolateral prefrontal cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(47), 17260–17268. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.1193-10.2011
  • Wilke, M., Holland, S. K., Altaye, M., & Gaser, C. (2008). Template-O-Matic: A toolbox for creating customized pediatric templates. Neuroimage, 41(3), 903–913. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.02.056
  • Zacks, J. M. (2008). Neuroimaging studies of mental rotation: A meta-analysis and review. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 1–19. doi:10.1162/jocn.2008.20013