2,353
Views
9
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Moving arms: the effects of sensorimotor information on the problem-solving process

, &
Pages 171-191 | Received 28 Apr 2016, Accepted 02 Jun 2018, Published online: 21 Nov 2018

References

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645.
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2016). Situated conceptualization: Theory and application. In Y. Coello & M. H. Fischer (Eds.), Foundations of embodied cognition, volume 1: Perceptual and emotional embodiment (pp. 11–37). London: Taylor & Francis.
  • Berch, D. B., Foley, E. J., Hill, R. J. & Ryan, P. M. (1999). Extracting parity and magnitude from Arabic numerals: developmental changes in number processing and mental representation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74, 286–308.
  • Chemero, A. (2009). Radical embodied cognitive sciences. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Coello, Y., & Fischer, M. H. (2016). Foundations of embodied cognition – volume 1. Perceptual and emotional embodiment. London: Taylor & Francis.
  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). Using another’s gaze as an explicit aid to insight problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371–396.
  • Dijkstra, K., Kaschak, M. P., & Zwaan, R. A. (2007). Body posture facilitates retrieval of autobiographical memories. Cognition, 102, 139–149.
  • Domahs, F., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H. C. (2010). Embodied numerosity: Implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition, 116, 251–266.
  • Duncker, K. (1963). Zur Psychologie des produktiven Denkens [The psychology of productive thinking]. Berlin: Julius Springer. (Original work published 1935).
  • Fischer, M. H. (2012). A hierarchical view of grounded, embodied and situated numerical cognition. Cognitive Processing, 13, S161–S164.
  • Fischer M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial-numerical associations point to finger counting as prime example of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 260. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260
  • Fischer, M. H., Castel, A. D., Dodd, M. D., & Pratt, J. (2003). Perceiving numbers causes spatial shifts of attention. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 555–556.
  • Fischer, M. H., & Coello, Y. (2016). Foundations of embodied cognition – volume 2: Conceptual and interactive embodiment. London: Taylor & Francis Ltd.
  • Fischer, M. H., & Knops, A. (2014). Attentional cueing in numerical cognition. Frontiers in Psychology. 5, 1381. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00987.
  • Fischer, M. H., & Shaki, S. (2014). Spatial associations in numerical cognition – from single digits to arithmetic. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1461–1483.
  • Göbel, S. M., Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H. (2011). The cultural number line: A review of cultural and linguistic influences on the development of number processing. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 42, 543–565.
  • Grant, E. R., & Spivey, M. J. (2003). Eye movements and problem solving: Guiding attention guides thought. Psychological Science, 14, 462–466.
  • Han, S., & Northoff, G. (2008). Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews: Neuroscience 9:646–654.
  • Jacob, P. (2016). Assessing raical embodiment. In Y. Coello & M. H. Fischer (Eds.), Foundations of embodied cognition, volume 1: Perceptual and emotional embodiment (pp. 38–58). London: Taylor & Francis.
  • Katona, G. (1940). Organizing and memorizing: Studies in the psychology of learning and teaching. New York, NY: Columbia University.
  • Kazandjian, S., Cavézian, C., Zivotofsky, A. Z., & Chokron, S. (2010). Bisections in two languages: When number processing, spatial representation, and habitual reading direction interact. Neuropsychologia, 48, 4031–4037.
  • Knops, A., Thirion, B., Hubbard, E. M., Michel, V., & Dehaene, S. (2009). Recruitment of an area involved in eye movements during mental arithmetic. Science, 324, 1583–1585.
  • Krause, F., & Lindemann, O. (2014). Expyriment: a Python library for cognitive and neuroscientific experiments. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 416–428.
  • Litchfield, D., & Ball, L. (2011). Using another’s gaze as an explicit aid to insight problem solving. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 649–656.
  • Liu, D., Cai, D., Verguts, T., & Chen, Q. (2017). The time course of spatial attention shifts in elementary arithmetic. Scientific Reports, 7, 921.
  • Luchins, A. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving. Psychological Monographs, 54, 1–22.
  • Maass, A., Suitner, C., & Deconchy, J. P. (2014). Living in an asymmetrical world: How writing direction affects thought and action. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Masson, N., & Pesenti, M. (2014). Attentional bias induced by solving simple and complex addition and subtraction problems. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1–13.
  • Newell, A., & Simon, H. A. (1972). Human problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  • Ninaus, M., Moeller, K., Kaufmann, L., Fischer, M. H., Nuerk, H. C., & Wood, G. (2017). Cognitive mechanisms underlying directional and non-directional spatial-numerical associations across the lifespan. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01421
  • Öllinger, M., Jones, G., & Knoblich, G. (2013). The dynamics of search, impasse, and representational change provide a coherent explanation of difficulty in the nine-dot problem. Psychological Research, 78, 266–275.
  • Öllinger, M., Jones, G., & Knoblich, G. (2014). Insight and search in Katona’s five-square problem. Experimental Psychology, 61, 263–272.
  • Pinhas, M., Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2014). Heed the signs: Operation signs have spatial associations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1–14.
  • Pizzera, A., & Raab, M. (2012). Perceptual judgments of sports officials are influenced by their motor and visual experience. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 24, 59–72.
  • Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110–122.
  • Raab, M., Johnson, J. G., & Heekeren, H. (Eds.). (2009). Mind and motion: The bidirectional link between thought and action. Progress in Brain Research, 174, 1–332.
  • Rizzolatti, G., Riggio, L., Dascola, I., & Umiltá, C. (1987). Reorienting attention across the horizontal and vertical meridians: Evidence in favor of a premotor theory of attention. Neuropsychologia, 25, 31–40.
  • Shaki, S., & Fischer, M. H. (2008). Reading space into numbers – A cross-linguistic comparison of the SNARC effect. Cognition, 108(2), 590–599.
  • Shaki, S., Fischer, M. H., & Petrusic W. M. (2009). Reading habits for both words and numbers contribute to the SNARC effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 328–331.
  • Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2007). Moving eyes and moving thought: On the spatial compatibility between eye movements and cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 663.
  • Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009a). Covert shifts of attention function as an implicit aid to insight. Cognition, 111, 168–174.
  • Thomas, L. E., & Lleras, A. (2009b). Swinging into thought: Directed movement guides insight in problem solving. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 649–723.
  • Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2013). Moving to solution: Effects of movement priming on problem solving. Experimental Psychology, 60, 403–409.
  • Werner, K., & Raab, M. (2014). Moving your eyes to solution: Effects of movements on the perception of a problem-solving task. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1571–1578.