1,182
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

When beliefs and evidence collide: psychological and ideological predictors of motivated reasoning about climate change

&
Pages 428-464 | Received 07 Sep 2020, Accepted 09 Oct 2021, Published online: 28 Oct 2021

References

  • Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brenswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (2019). The authoritarian personality. Verso Books.
  • Allport, G. W., & Ross, J. M. (1967). Personal religious orientation and prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 5(4), 432–443.
  • Altemeyer, B. (1996). Dogmatism. In B. Altemeyer (Ed.), The authoritarian specter. Harvard University Press.
  • Altemeyer, B. (2002). Dogmatic behavior among students: Testing a new measure of dogmatism. The Journal of Social Psychology, 142(6), 713–721. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224540209603931
  • Amit, A., & Sagiv, L. (2013). The role of epistemic motivation in individuals’ response to decision complexity. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 121(1), 104–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2013.01.003
  • Appelbaum, A. (2020). Twilight of democracy: The seductive lure of authoritarianism. Penguin.
  • Azarian, B. (2015, February 25). Spreading pseudoscience: 5 reasons why some liberals are as bad as conservatives. Huffington Post. http://www.huffingtonpost.com/bobby-azarian/spreading-pseudoscience-5-reasons-liberals_b_6694374.html
  • Ball, L., Thompson, V. A., & Stupple, E. J. N. (2018). Conflict and dual process theory: The case of belief bias. In W. De Neys (Ed.), Dual process theory 2.0 (pp. 100–120). Routledge.
  • Baum, M. A., & Potter, P. B. (2019). Media, public opinion, and foreign policy in the age of social media. The Journal of Politics, 81(2), 747–756. https://doi.org/10.1086/702233
  • Bayes, R., & Druckman, J. N. (2021). Motivated reasoning and climate change. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 42, 27–35. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2021.02.009
  • Berinsky, A. J., Huber, G. A., & Lenz, G. S. (2012). Evaluating online labor markets for experimental research: Amazon.com’s Mechanical Turk. Political Analysis, 20(3), 351–368. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpr057
  • Billeh, V. Y., & Zakhariades, G. A. (1975). The development and application of a scale for measuring scientific attitudes. Science Education, 59(2), 155–165. https://doi.org/10.1002/sce.3730590203
  • Bode, L., & Vraga, E. K. (2015). In related news, that was wrong: The correction of misinformation through related stories functionality in social media. Journal of Communication, 65(4), 619–638. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12166
  • Boykoff, M. T. (2008). Lost in translation? United States television news coverage of anthropogenic climate change, 1995–2004. Climatic Change, 86(1–2), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-007-9299-3
  • Boykoff, M. T., & Boykoff, J. M. (2004). Balance as bias: Global warming and the US prestige press. Global Environmental Change, 14(2), 125–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2003.10.001
  • Bronstein, M. V., Pennycook, G., Joormann, J., Corlett, P. R., & Cannon, T. D. (2019). Dual-process theory, conflict processing, and delusional belief. Clinical Psychology Review, 72, 101748.
  • Brüggemann, M., & Engesser, S. (2017). Beyond false balance: How interpretive journalism shapes media coverage of climate change. Global Environmental Change, 42, 58–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2016.11.004
  • Buhrmester, M., Kwang, T., & Gosling, S. D. (2011). Amazon’s Mechanical Turk: A new source of inexpensive, yet high quality, data? Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 6(1), 3–5.
  • Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need for cognition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 42(1), 116–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.42.1.116
  • Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., & Kao, C. F. (1984). The efficient assessment of need for cognition. Journal of Personality Assessment, 48(3), 306–307. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327752jpa4803_13
  • Caddick, Z. (2015). Brief climate change belief questionnaire. Unpublished. San Jose State University.
  • Čavojová, V., Šrol, J., & Jurkovič, M. (2020). Why should we try to think like scientists? Scientific reasoning and susceptibility to epistemically suspect beliefs and cognitive biases. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34(1), 85–95. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3595
  • Chmielewski, M., & Kucker, S. C. (2020). An MTurk crisis? Shifts in data quality and the impact on study results. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(4), 464–473. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619875149
  • Cillizzia, C. (2021, July 12). Donald Trump just accidently told the truth about his view on polls. CNN. Retrieved July 12, 2021, from https://www.cnn.com/2021/07/12/politics/donald-trump-polls-cpac-straw-poll/index.html
  • Clifford, S., Jewell, R. M., & Waggoner, P. D. (2015). Are samples drawn from Mechanical Turk valid for research on political ideology? Research & Politics, 4(2), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168015622072
  • Conservatism. (n.d.). Retrieved online August 7, 2017 from http://Merriam-Webster.com.
  • Cook, J., Nuccitelli, D., Green, S. A., Richardson, M., Winkler, B., Painting, R., Way, R., Jacobs, P., & Skuce, A. (2013). Quantifying the consensus on anthropogenic global warming in the scientific literature. Environmental Research Letters, 8(2), 024024. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/8/2/024024
  • Corbett, J. B., & Durfee, J. L. (2004). Testing public (un)certainty of science: Media representations of global warming. Science Communication, 26(2), 129–151. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547004270234
  • Crowson, H. M. (2009). Does the DOG scale measure dogmatism? Another look at construct validity. The Journal of Social Psychology, 149(3), 365–383. https://doi.org/10.3200/SOCP.149.3.365-383
  • Crowson, H. M., DeBacker, T. K., & Davis, K. A. (2008). The DOG Scale: A valid measure of dogmatism? Journal of Individual Differences, 29(1), 17–24. https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001.29.1.17
  • De Neys, W. (2012). Bias and conflict: A case for logical intuitions. Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 7(1), 28–38. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691611429354
  • De Neys, W. (Ed.). (2018). Dual process theory 2.0. Routledge.
  • Dennis, B., Mufson, S., & Clement, S. (2019, September 13). Americans increasingly see climate change as a crisis, poll says. The Washington Post. Retrieved July 10, 2021, from https://www.washingtonpost.com/climate-environment/americans-increasingly-see-climate-change-as-a-crisis-poll-shows/2019/09/12/74234db0-cd2a-11e9-87fa-8501a456c003_story.html
  • Digman, J. M., & Inouye, J. (1986). Further specification of the five robust factors of personality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(1), 116–123. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.1.116
  • Druckman, J. N., & McGrath, M. C. (2019). The evidence for motivated reasoning in climate change preference formation. Nature Climate Change, 9(2), 111–119. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41558-018-0360-1
  • Du, S., & Gregory, S. (2016, November). The Echo Chamber Effect in Twitter: Does community polarization increase? In H. Cherifi, S. Gaito, W. Quattrociocchi, & A. Sala (Eds.), International workshop on complex networks and their applications (pp. 373–378). Springer.
  • Duckitt, J., Bizumic, B., Krauss, S. W., & Heled, E. (2010). A tripartite approach to right-wing authoritarianism: The authoritarianism-conservatism-traditionalism model. Political Psychology, 31(5), 685–715. [Database] https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9221.2010.00781.x
  • Epstein, S. (1990). Cognitive– Experiential self theory. In L. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of personality theory and research (pp. 165–192). Guilford.
  • Epstein, S. (1994). Integration of the cognitive and the psychodynamic unconscious. The American Psychologist, 49(8), 709–724. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.49.8.709
  • Evans, J. S. B. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093629
  • Evans, J. T. (2002). The influence of prior belief on scientific thinking. In P. Carruthers, S. Stich, & M. Siegal (Eds.), The cognitive basis of science (pp. 193–210). Cambridge University Press.
  • Evans, J. T. (2011). Dual-process theories of reasoning: Contemporary issues and developmental applications. Developmental Review, 31(2–3), 86–102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2011.07.007
  • Evans, J. T., & Over, D. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Psychology Press.
  • Feist, G. J. (2006). The psychology of science and the origins of the scientific mind. Yale University Press.
  • Feist, G. J. (2012). Predicting interest in and attitudes toward science from personality and need for cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(7), 771–775. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.01.005
  • Fraser, B. J. (1981). Test of science related attitudes. Educational Testing Service.
  • Freud, S. (1900/1981). Die traumdeutung [Interpretation of dreams]. Fisher Verlag. (Original work published 1900)
  • Funk, C., & Kennedy, B. (2020). How Americans see climate change and the environment in 7 charts. Pew Research Center Fact Tank. Retrieved June 17, 2021, from https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2020/04/21/how-americans-see-climate-change-and-the-environment-in-7-charts/
  • Gallup. (2016, March 16). U.S. concern about global warming at eight-year high. http://www.gallup.com/poll/190010/concern-global-warming-eight-year-high.aspx
  • Gorman, M. E. (1992). Simulating science: Heuristics, mental models, and technoscientific thinking. Indiana University Press.
  • Gray, S. J., & Gallo, D. A. (2016). Paranormal psychic believers and skeptics: A large-scale test of the cognitive differences hypothesis. Memory & Cognition, 44(2), 242–261.
  • Gwet, K. L. (2008). Computing inter-rater reliability and its variance in the presence of high agreement. The British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology, 61(Pt 1), 29–48. https://doi.org/10.1348/000711006X126600
  • Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.4.814
  • Hart, P. S., & Nisbet, E. C. (2012). Boomerang effects in science communication: How motivated reasoning and identity cues amplify opinion polarization about climate mitigation policies. Communication Research, 39(6), 701–723. https://doi.org/10.1177/0093650211416646
  • Huff, C., & Tingley, D. (2015). Who are these people? Evaluating the demographic characteristics and political preferences of MTurk survey respondents. Research & Politics, 2(3), 205316801560464. https://doi.org/10.1177/2053168015604648
  • Jern, A., Chang, K. M. K., & Kemp, C. (2014). Belief polarization is not always irrational. Psychological Review, 121(2), 206–224. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035941
  • John, O. P., Naumann, L. P., & Soto, C. J. (2008). Paradigm shift to the integrative Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and conceptual issues. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of creativity (3rd ed., pp. 114–158). Cambridge University Press; Guilford Press.
  • John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five Trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theoretical perspectives. In L.A. Pervin & O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 102–138). Guilford.
  • Kahan, D. M. (2012). Ideology, motivated reasoning, and cognitive reflection: An experimental study. Judgment and Decision Making, 8, 407–424.
  • Kahan, D. M. (2015a). Climate‐science communication and the measurement problem. Political Psychology, 36, 1–43. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12244
  • Kahan, D. M. (2015b). The politically motivated reasoning paradigm, Part 1: What politically motivated reasoning is and how to measure it. In R. Scott & S. Kosslyn (Eds.), Emerging trends in the social and behavioral sciences: An interdisciplinary, searchable, and linkable resource (pp. 1–16). John Wiley & Sons.
  • Kahan, D. M., Peters, E., Wittlin, M., Slovic, P., Ouellette, L. L., Braman, D., & Mandel, G. (2012). The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nature Climate Change, 2(10), 732–735. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1547
  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.
  • Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. The Psychology of Intuitive Judgment, 49, 49–81.
  • Kardash, C. M., & Scholes, R. J. (1996). Effects of preexisting beliefs, epistemological beliefs, and need for cognition on interpretation of controversial issues. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(2), 260–271. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.88.2.260
  • Kim, J., Tabibian, B., Oh, A., Schölkopf, B., & Gomez-Rodriguez, M. (2018, February). Leveraging the crowd to detect and reduce the spread of fake news and misinformation. In Proceedings of the Eleventh ACM International Conference on Web Search and Data Mining (pp. 324–332). ACM. https://doi.org/10.1145/3159652.3159734
  • Klaczynski, P. A. (1997). Bias in adolescents’ everyday reasoning and its relationship with intellectual ability, personal theories, and self-serving motivation. Developmental Psychology, 33(2), 273–283. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.33.2.273
  • Klaczynski, P. A., & Narasimham, G. (1998). Development of scientific reasoning biases: Cognitive versus ego-protective explanations. Developmental Psychology, 34(1), 175–187.
  • Koslowski, B. (1996). Theory and evidence: The development of scientific reasoning. MIT Press.
  • Kovaka, K. (2021). Climate change denial and beliefs about science. Synthese, 198(3), 2355–2374. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02210-z
  • Kruglanski, A. W., Jasko, K., & Friston, K. (2020). All thinking is 'Wishful' Thinking. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(6), 413–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2020.03.004
  • Kruglanski, A. W., & Klar, Y. (1987). A view from a bridge: Synthesizing the consistency and attribution paradigms from a lay epistemic perspective. European Journal of Social Psychology, 17(2), 211–241. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420170208
  • Kunda, Z. (1990). The case for motivated reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108(3), 480–498. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.108.3.480
  • Lacap, M. P. (2015). The scientific attitudes of students major in science in the new teacher education curriculum. Asia Pacific Journal of Multidisciplinary Research, 3(5), 7–15.
  • Lewandowsky, S. (2021). Climate change disinformation and how to combat it. Annual Review of Public Health, 42, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-publhealth-090419-102409
  • Lindeman, M. (1998). Motivation, cognition and pseudoscience. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 39(4), 257–265. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9450.00085
  • Lobato, E. J. C., & Zimmerman, C. (2018). Examining how people reason about controversial scientific topics. Thinking and Reasoning, 67, 1–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00376456
  • Lombardi, D., Nussbaum, E. M., & Sinatra, G. M. (2016). Plausibility judgments in conceptual change and epistemic cognition. Educational Psychologist, 51(1), 35–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2015.1113134
  • Mandel, D. R. (2014). The psychology of Bayesian reasoning. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 1144. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01144
  • McAuliff, B. D., & Kovera, M. B. (2008). Juror need for cognition and sensitivity to methodological flaws in expert evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 38(2), 385–408. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2007.00310.x
  • McCright, A. M. (2011). Political orientation moderates Americans’ beliefs and concern about climate change. Climatic Change, 104(2), 243–253. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-010-9946-y
  • McCright, A. M., & Dunlap, R. E. (2011). The politicization of climate change and polarization in the American public’s views of global warming, 2001–2010. The Sociological Quarterly, 52(2), 155–194. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1533-8525.2011.01198.x
  • Mercier, H., & Heintz, C. (2014). Scientists’ argumentative reasoning. Topoi, 33(2), 513–524.
  • Messick, S. (1976). Individuality in learning. Jossey-Bass.
  • Messick, S. (1984). The nature of cognitive styles: Problems and promise in educational practice. Educational Psychologist, 19(2), 59–74. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461528409529283
  • Mooney, C. (2011, May/June). The science of why we don’t believe science. Mother Jones, 11p. https://www.motherjones.com/politics/2011/04/denial-science-chris-mooney/
  • Moss, A. J., Rosenzweig, C., Robinson, J., Jaffe, S. N., & Litman, L. (2020, April 28). Is it ethical to use Mechanical Turk for behavioral research? Relevant data from a representative survey of MTurk participants and wages. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/jbc9d
  • Neuberg, S. L., Judice, T. N., & West, S. G. (1997). What the need for closure scale measures and what it does not: Toward differentiating among related epistemic motives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 1396–1412. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.72.6.1396
  • Nickerson, R. S. (1998). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.2.2.175
  • Paolacci, G., Chandler, J., & Ipeirotis, P. G. (2010). Running experiments on amazon mechanical turk. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(5), 411–419.
  • Paolacci, G., & Chandler, J. (2014). Inside the Turk: Understanding Mechanical Turk as a participant pool. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 184–188. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414531598
  • Pennycook, G., Cheyne, J. A., Barr, N., Koehler, D. J., & Fugelsang, J. A. (2014). Cognitive style and religiosity: The role of conflict detection. Memory & Cognition, 42(1), 1–10. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0340-7
  • Pennycook, G., McPhetres, J., Zhang, Y., Lu, J. G., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Fighting COVID-19 misinformation on social media: Experimental evidence for a scalable accuracy-nudge intervention. Psychological Science, 31(7), 770–780. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797620939054
  • Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2019). Cognitive reflection and the 2016 U.S. Presidential election. Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(2), 224–239. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167218783192
  • Petersen, A. M., Vincent, E. M., & Westerling, A. L. (2019). Discrepancy in scientific authority and media visibility of climate change scientists and contrarians. Nature Communications, 10(1), 1–14.
  • Peterson, R. A., & Merunka, D. R. (2019). Convenience samples of college students and research reproducibility. Journal of Business Research, 67(5), 1035–1041. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbusres.2013.08.010
  • Pew Research Center. (2015, January 29). What Americans and scientists think about science. https://www.pewresearch.org/fact-tank/2015/01/29/5-key-findings-science/
  • Pew Research Center. (2016, October 4). 1. Public views on climate change and climate scientists. https://www.pewresearch.org/politics/2012/10/15/more-say-there-is-solid-evidence-of-global-warming/
  • Ranney, M. A., & Clark, D. (2016). Climate change conceptual change: Scientific information can transform attitudes. Topics in Cognitive Science, 8(1), 49–75.
  • Ripberger, J. T., Jenkins-Smith, H. C., Silva, C. L., Carlson, D. E., Gupta, K., Carlson, N., & Dunlap, R. E. (2017). Bayesian versus politically motivated reasoning in human perception of climate anomalies. Environmental Research Letters, 12(11), 114004. https://doi.org/10.1088/1748-9326/aa8cfc
  • Sadowski, C. J., & Cogburn, H. E. (1997). Need for cognition in the big-five factor structure. The Journal of Psychology, 131(3), 307–312. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223989709603517
  • Sadowski, C. J., & Gulgoz, S. (1992). Internal consistency and test-retest reliability of the Need for Cognition Scale. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 74(2), 610–610. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1992.74.2.610
  • Sinatra, G. M., Kienhues, D., & Hofer, B. K. (2014). Addressing challenges to public understanding of science: Epistemic cognition, motivated reasoning, and conceptual change. Educational Psychologist, 49(2), 123–138. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2014.916216
  • Sinatra, G. M., & Lombardi, D. (2020). Evaluating sources of scientific evidence and claims in the post-truth era may require reappraising plausibility judgments. Educational Psychologist, 55(3), 120–131. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2020.1730181
  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. The Free Press.
  • Slothuus, R., & De Vreese, C. H. (2010). Political parties, motivated reasoning, and issue framing effects. The Journal of Politics, 72(3), 630–645. https://doi.org/10.1017/S002238161000006X
  • Speers, T., & Lewis, J. (2004). Journalists and jabs: Media coverage of the MMR vaccine. Communication & Medicine, 1(2), 171–181.
  • Stanley, S. K., & Wilson, M. S. (2019). Meta-analysing the association between social dominance orientation, authoritarianism, and attitudes on the environment and climate change. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 61, 46–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jenvp.2018.12.002
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1997). Reasoning independently of prior belief and individual differences in actively open-minded thinking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(2), 342–357. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.2.342
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1998). Individual differences in rational thought. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 127(2), 161–188. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.127.2.161
  • Stanovich, K. E., West, R. F., & Toplak, M. E. (2016). The rationality quotient: Toward a test of rational thinking. MIT Press.
  • Stenner, K. (2009). Three kinds of “conservatism”. Psychological Inquiry, 20(2–3), 142–159. https://doi.org/10.1080/10478400903028615
  • Stern, C., & Axt, J. (2020). Investigating whether group status modulates the relationship between individual differences in epistemic motivation and political conservatism. Journal of Research in Personality, 86, 103940. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2020.103940
  • Tappin, B. M., Pennycook, G., & Rand, D. G. (2020). Thinking clearly about causal inferences of politically motivated reasoning: Why paradigmatic study designs often undermine causal inference. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 34, 81–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2020.01.003
  • Thagard, P. (2004). Rationality and science. In A. R. Mele & R. Rawling (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of rationality (pp. 363–379). Oxford University Press.
  • Thomas, K. A., & Clifford, S. (2017). Validity and Mechanical Turk: An assessment of exclusion methods and interactive experiments. Computers in Human Behavior, 77, 184–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.038
  • Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1971). Belief in the law of small numbers. Psychological Bulletin, 76(2), 105–110. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0031322
  • Tweney, R. (1998). Toward a cognitive psychology of science: Recent research and its implications. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(5), 150–154. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10836837
  • Van Bavel, J. J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., Crockett, M. J., Crum, A. J., Douglas, K. M., Druckman, J. N., Drury, J., Dube, O., Ellemers, N., Finkel, E. J., Fowler, J. H., Gelfand, M., Han, S., Haslam, S. A., … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4, 460–471.
  • Ward, T., & Garety, P. A. (2019). Fast and slow thinking in distressing delusions: A review of the literature and implications for targeted therapy. Schizophrenia Research, 203, 80–87.
  • West, R. F., Meserve, R. J., & Stanovich, K. E. (2012). Cognitive sophistication does not attenuate the bias blind spot. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 103(3), 506–519. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028857
  • West, R. F., Toplak, M. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (2008). Heuristics and biases as measures of critical thinking: Associations with cognitive ability and thinking dispositions. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100(4), 930–941. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012842
  • Woo, S. E., Harms, P. D., & Kuncel, N. R. (2007). Integrating personality and intelligence: Typical intellectual engagement and need for cognition. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(6), 1635–1639. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.022

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.