Publication Cover
Laterality
Asymmetries of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognition
Volume 20, 2015 - Issue 4
303
Views
6
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Environmental script affects lateral asymmetry of word recognition: A study of French–Hebrew bilinguals tested in Israel and in France

&
Pages 389-417 | Received 23 Jul 2014, Accepted 11 Nov 2014, Published online: 12 Dec 2014

REFERENCES

  • Adamson, M. M., & Hellige, J. B. (2006). Hemispheric differences for identification of words and nonwords in Urdu–English bilinguals. Neuropsychology, 20, 232–248. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.20.2.232
  • Andrews, B., Aisenberg, D., d'Avossa, G., & Sapir, A. (2013). Cross-cultural effects on the assumed light source direction: Evidence from English and Hebrew readers. Journal of Vision, 13, 2. doi:10.1167/13.13.2
  • Babkoff, H., Faust, M., & Lavidor, M. (1997). Lexical decision, visual hemifield and angle of orientation. Neuropsychologia, 35, 487–495. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00072-3
  • Barber, H. A., Ben-Zvi, S., Bentin, S., & Kutas, M. (2001). Parafoveal perception during sentence reading? An ERP paradigm using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) with flankers. Psychophysiology, 48, 525–531. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01082.x
  • Barrett, A. B., Kim, M., Crucian, G. P., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). Spatial bias: Effects of early reading direction on Korean subjects. Neuropsychologia, 40, 1003–1012. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00147-6
  • Barton, M. I., Goodglass, H., & Shai, A. (1965). Differential recognition of tachistoscopically presented English and Hebrew words in right and left visual fields. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 21, 431–437. doi:10.2466/pms.1965.21.2.431
  • Bentin, S. (1981). On the representation of a second language in the cerebral hemispheres of right-handed people. Neuropsychology, 19, 599–603. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(81)90028-2
  • Bolger, D. J., Perfetti, C. A., & Schneider, W. (2005). Cross-cultural effect on the brain revisited: Universal structures plus writing system variation. Human Brain Mapping, 25, 92–104. doi:10.1002/hbm.20124
  • Bourne, V. J. (2006). The divided visual field paradigm: Methodological considerations. Laterality, 11, 373–393. doi:10.1080/13576500600633982
  • Bricout-Tomasi, L., Billard, C., & Siéroff, E. (2010). Absence of right visual field attentional bias in reading in dyslexic children. Behavioural Neurology, 23, 189–191. doi:10.3233/BEN-2010-0291
  • Bryden, M. P. (1961). The role of post-exposural eye movements in tachistoscopic perception. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 15, 220–225. doi:10.1037/h0083445
  • Carmon, A., Nachshon, I., & Starinsky, R. (1976). Developmental aspects of visual hemifield differences in perception of verbal material. Brain and Language, 3, 463–469. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(76)90041-9
  • Chokron, S. (2002). On the origin of free-viewing perceptual asymmetries. Cortex, 38, 109–112. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70644-0
  • Chokron, S., & De Agostini, M. (1995). Line bisection among French and Israeli subjects: A developmental approach. Brain Research: Cognitive, 3, 51–58. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(95)00018-6
  • Chokron, S., & De Agostini, M. (2000). Reading habits influence aesthetic preference. Brain Research: Cognitive, 10, 45–49. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00021-5
  • Chokron, S., & Imbert, M. (1993). Reading habits and line bisection. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 219–222. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(93)90005-P
  • Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Lehéricy, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Henaff, M. A., & Michel, F. (2000). The visual word form area: Spatial and temporal characterization of an initial stage of reading in normal subjects and posterior split-brain patients. Brain, 123, 291–307. doi:10.1093/brain/123.2.291
  • Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Vinckier, F., Jobert, A., & Montavont, A. (2008). Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways. NeuroImage, 40, 353–366. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.036
  • Content, A., Mousty, P., & Radeau, M. (1990). Brulex. Une base de données lexicales informatisées pour le français écrit et parlé [Brulex. A computerized lexical database for written and spoken French]. L'Année Psychologique, 90, 551–566. doi:10.3406/psy.1991.29428
  • Corbetta, M., & Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 201–215. doi:10.1038/nrn755
  • Dehaene, S., Pegado, F., Braga, L. W., Ventura, P., Nunes Filho, G., Jobert, A., … Cohen, L. (2010). How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science, 330, 1359–1364. doi:10.1126/science.1194140
  • Deutsch, A., & Rayner, K. (1999). Initial fixation location effects in reading Hebrew words. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 393–421. doi:10.1080/016909699386284
  • Ellis, A. W. (2004). Length, formats, hemispheres, and the processing of words presented laterally or at fixation. Brain and Language, 88, 355–366. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00166-4
  • Eviatar, Z. (1995). Reading direction and attention: Effects on lateralized ignoring. Brain and Cognition, 29, 137–150. doi:10.1006/brcg.1995.1273
  • Eviatar, Z. (1997). Language experience and right hemisphere tasks: The effects of scanning habits and multilingualism. Brain and Language, 58, 157–173. doi:10.1006/brln.1997.1863
  • Eviatar, Z. (1999). Cross-language tests of hemispheric strategies in reading nonwords. Neuropsychology, 13, 498–515. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.13.4.498
  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2004). Morphological and orthographic effects on hemispheric processing of nonwords: A cross-linguistic comparison. Reading and Writing, 17, 691–705. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2659-8
  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2007). Morphological structure and hemispheric functioning: The contribution of the right hemisphere to reading in different languages. Neuropsychology, 21, 470–484. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.4.470
  • Fagard, J., & Dahmen, R. (2003). The effects of reading–writing direction on the asymmetry of space perception and directional tendencies: A comparison between French and Tunisian children. Laterality, 8, 39–52. doi:10.1080/713754473
  • Farid, M., & Grainger, J. (1996). How initial fixation position influences visual word recognition: A comparison of French and Arabic. Brain and Language, 53, 351–368. doi:10.1006/brln.1996.0053
  • Fernandino, L., Iacoboni, M., & Zaidel, E. (2007). The effects of bilateral presentations on lateralized lexical decision. Brain and Cognition, 64, 60–67. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2006.11.004
  • Fink, G. R., Driver, J., Rorden, C., Baldeweg, T., & Dolan, R. J. (2000). Neural consequences of competing stimuli in both visual hemifields: A physiological basis for visual extinction. Annals of Neurology, 47, 440–446. doi:10.1002/1531-8249(200004)47:4<440::AID-ANA6>3.0.CO;2-E
  • Forgays, D. G. (1953). The development of differential word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 45, 165–168. doi:10.1037/h0053888
  • Frost, R., & Plaut, D. C. (2001). The word-frequency database for printed Hebrew. Retrieved from http://word-freq.mscc.huji.ac.il/
  • Gatheron, D., & Siéroff, E. (1999). Right hemifield superiority in reading and attentional factors. Brain and Cognition, 40, 122–125. doi:10.1006/brcg.1998.1024
  • Geldmacher, S. D., & Alhaj, M. (1999). Spatial aspects of letter cancellation performance in Arabic readers. International Journal of Neuroscience, 97, 29–39. doi:10.3109/00207459908994301
  • Ghent Braine, L. (1968). Asymmetries of pattern perception observed in Israelis. Neuropsychologia, 6, 73–88. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(68)90041-9
  • Gilbert, C., & Bakan, P. (1973). Visual asymmetry in perception of faces. Neuropsychologia, 11, 355–362. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(73)90049-3
  • Häikiö, T., Bertram, R., Hyönä, J., & Niemi, P. (2009). Development of the letter identity span in reading: Evidence from the eye movement moving window paradigm. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 167–181. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.002
  • Hardyck, C., Chiarello, C., Dronkers, N. F., & Simpson, G. V. (1985). Orienting attention within visual fields: How efficient is interhemispheric transfer? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 11, 650–666. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.11.5.650
  • Heath, R., Rouhana, A., & Ghanem, D. A. (2005). Asymmetric bias in perception of facial affect among Roman and Arabic script readers. Laterality, 10, 51–64. doi:10.1080/13576500342000293
  • Hellige, J. B., Taylor, A. K., & Eng, T. L. (1989). Interhemispheric interaction when both hemispheres have access to the same stimulus information. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 15, 711–722. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.15.4.711
  • Hellige, J. B., & Yamauchi, M. (1999). Quantitative and qualitative hemispheric asymmetry for processing Japanese Kana. Brain and Cognition, 40, 453–463. doi:10.1006/brcg.1999.1074
  • Heron, W. (1957). Perception as a function of retinal locus and attention. The American Journal of Psychology, 70, 38–48. doi:10.2307/1419227
  • Hunter, Z. R., & Brysbaert, M. (2008). Visual half-field experiments are a good measure of cerebral language dominance if used properly: Evidence from fMRI. Neuropsychologia, 46, 316–325. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.07.007
  • Iacoboni, M., & Zaidel, E. (1996). Hemispheric independence in word recognition: Evidence from unilateral and bilateral presentations. Brain and Language, 53, 121–140. doi:10.1006/brln.1996.0040
  • Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2009). Language status and hemispheric involvement in reading: Evidence from trilingual Arabic speakers tested in Arabic, Hebrew, and English. Neuropsychology, 23, 240–254. doi:10.1037/a0014193
  • Ibrahim, R., Israeli, N., & Eviatar, Z. (2010). Hemispheric involvement in reading: The effects of language experience. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23, 427–442. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2010.04.002
  • Joseph, H. S. S. L., Liversedge, S. P., Blythe, H. I., White, S. J., & Rayner, K. (2009). Word length and landing position effects during reading in children and adults. Vision Research, 49, 2078–2086. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2009.05.015
  • Kazandjian, S., Gaash, E., Love, I. Y., Zivotofsky, A. Z., & Chokron, S. (2011). Spatial representation of action phrases among bidirectional readers. Social Psychology, 42, 249–258. doi:10.1027/1864-9335/a000069
  • Kimura, D. (1966). Dual functional asymmetry of the brain in visual perception. Neuropsychologia, 4, 275–285. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(66)90033-9
  • Kinsbourne, M. (1970). The cerebral basis of lateral asymmetries in attention. Acta Psychologica, 33, 193–201. doi:10.1016/0001-6918(70)90132-0
  • Lefavrais, P. (1963). Du diagnostic de la dyslexie à l'étude clinique de la lecture. Un nouvel instrument: le test de “l'Alouette” [From the diagnosis of dyslexia to the clinical study of reading. A new tool: The “Alouette” test]. Revue de Psychologie Appliquée, 13, 189–207.
  • Leon-Carrion, J., Garcia-Orza, J., & Perez-Santamaria, F. J. (2004). Development of the inhibitory component of the executive functions in children and adolescents. International Journal of Neuroscience, 114, 1291–1311. doi:10.1080/00207450490476066
  • Levy, J., Heller, W., Banich, M. T., & Burton, L. A. (1983). Are variations among right-handed individuals in perceptual asymmetries caused by characteristic arousal differences between hemispheres? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 9, 329–359. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.9.3.329
  • Lonvig, A. (2013). Frederic the frog and Elias the elephant (1st ed.). Retrieved from http://www.lonvig.dk/frederic-english.htm
  • Lubow, R. E., Tsal, Y., Mirkin, A., & Mazliah, G. (1994). English and Hebrew letter report by English- and Hebrew-reading subjects: Evidence for stimulus control, not hemispheric asymmetry. Brain and Cognition, 25, 34–51. doi:10.1006/brcg.1994.1021
  • Maass, A., & Russo, A. (2003). Directional bias in the mental representation of spatial events: Nature or culture? Psychological Science, 14, 296–301. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.14421
  • Marcel, T., Katz, L., & Smith M. (1974). Laterality and reading proficiency. Neuropsychologia, 12, 131–139. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(74)90034-7
  • Marshall, J. C., Caplan, D., & Holmes, J. M. (1975). The measure of laterality. Neuropsychologia, 13, 315–321. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(75)90008-1
  • McCandliss, B. D., Cohen, L., & Dehaene, S. (2003). The visual word form area: Expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 293–299. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00134-7
  • Melamed, F., & Zaidel, E. (1993). Language and task effects on lateralized word recognition. Brain and Language, 45, 70–85. doi:10.1006/brln.1993.1034
  • Miller, L. K., & Turner, S. (1973). Development of hemifield differences in word recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 65, 172–176. doi:10.1037/h0034991
  • Mishkin, M., & Forgays, D. G. (1952). Word recognition as a function of retinal locus. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 43, 43–48. doi:10.1037/h0061361
  • Mondor, T. A., & Bryden, M. P. (1992). On the relation between visual spatial attention and visual field asymmetry. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 44A, 529–555. doi:10.3758/BF03206699
  • Morikawa, K., & Mcbeath, M. K. (1992). Lateral motion bias associated with reading direction. Vision Research, 32, 1137–1141. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(92)90014-A
  • Nachshon, I. (1985). Directional preferences in perception of visual stimuli. International Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 161–174. doi:10.3109/00207458508985369
  • Nazir, T. A., Ben-Boutayab, N., Decoppet, N., Deutsch, A., & Frost, R. (2004). Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words. Brain and Language, 88, 294–311. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00168-8
  • Neumann, M. M., Hood, M., & Ford, R. M. (2013). Using environmental print to enhance emergent literacy and print motivation. Reading and Writing, 26, 771–793. doi:10.1007/s11145-012-9390-7
  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & Roberts, G. R. (2002). Can free-viewing perceptual asymmetries be explained by scanning, pre-motor or attentional biases? Cortex, 38, 113–136. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70645-2
  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & Wood, A. G. (1998). The contribution of attention to the right visual field advantage for word recognition. Brain and Cognition, 38, 339–357. doi:10.1006/brcg.1998.1052
  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9, 97–113. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4
  • Olson, M. E. (1973). Laterality differences in tachistoscopic word recognition in normal and delayed readers in elementary school. Neuropsychologia, 11, 343–350. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(73)90047-X
  • Orbach, J. (1952). Retinal locus as a factor in the recognition of visually perceived words. The American Journal of Psychology, 65, 555–562. doi:10.2307/1418035
  • Orbach, J. (1967). Differential recognition of English and Hebrew words in right and left visual fields as a function of cerebral dominance and reading habits. Neuropsychologia, 5, 127–134. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(67)90014-0
  • Ouellet, M., Santiago, J., Israeli, Z., & Gabay, S. (2010). Is the future the right time? Experimental Psychology, 57, 308–314. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000036
  • Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A. D., & Rayner, K. (1981). Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli readers. Brain and Language, 14, 174–181. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(81)90073-0
  • Rayner, K. (1986). Eye movements and the perceptual span in beginning and skilled readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 211–236. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(86)90037-8
  • Rinaldi, L., Di Luca, S., Henik, A., & Girelli, L. (2014). Reading direction shifts visuospatial attention: An interactive account of attentional biases. Acta Psychologica, 151, 98–105. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.018
  • Riva, M., & Siéroff, E. (2010). Right visual field attentional bias in reading in children and adults. Behavioural Neurology, 23, 199–201. doi:10.3233/BEN-2010-0294
  • Shanon, B. (1982). Lateralization effects in the perception of Hebrew and English words. Brain and Language, 17, 107–123. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(82)90008-6
  • Siéroff, E., Dahmen, R., & Fagard, J. (2012). Mechanisms of attention in reading parafoveal words: A cross-linguistic study in children. Neuropsychology, 26, 334–346. doi:10.1037/a0027572
  • Siéroff, E., Pollatsek, A., & Posner, M. I. (1988). Recognition of visual letter strings following injury to the posterior visual spatial system. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 5, 427–449. doi:10.1080/02643298808253268
  • Siéroff, E., & Riva, M. (2011). Attention and the identification of parafoveal words in school-age children and adults. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 24, 420–434. doi:10.1016/j.jneuroling.2011.01.003
  • Siéroff, E., & Urbanski, M. (2002). Conditions of visual verbal extinction: Does the ipsilesional stimulus have to be identified? Brain and Cognition, 48, 563–569. doi:10.1006/brcg.2001.1418
  • Silverberg, R., Bentin, S., Gaziel, T., Obler, L. K., & Albert, M. L. (1979). Shift of visual field preference for English words in native Hebrew speakers. Brain and Language, 8, 184–190. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(79)90048-8
  • Silverberg, R., Gordon, H. W., Pollack, S., & Bentin, S. (1980). Shift of visual field preference for Hebrew words in native speakers learning to read. Brain and Language, 11, 99–105. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(80)90113-3
  • Smigasiewicz, K., Shalgi, S., Hsieh, S., Möller, F., Jaffe, S., Chang, C. C., & Verleger, R. (2010). Left visual-field advantage in the dual-stream RSVP task and reading-direction: A study in three nations. Neuropsychologia, 48, 2852–2860. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.05.027
  • Thiebaut de Schotten, M., Dell'Acqua, F., Forkel, S. J., Simmons, A., Vergani, F., Murphy, D. G. M., & Catani, M. (2011). A lateralized brain network for visuospatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 14, 1245–1246. doi:10.1038/nn.2905
  • Tramer, O., Butler, B. E., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (1985). Evidence for scanning with unilateral visual presentation of letters. Brain and Language, 25, 1–18. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(85)90117-8
  • Vaid, J., & Singh, M. (1989). Asymmetries in the perception of facial affect: Is there an influence of reading habits? Neuropsychologia, 27, 1277–1287. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(89)90040-7
  • Vera, D. (2011). Using popular culture print to increase emergent literacy skills in one high-poverty urban school district. Journal of Early Childhood Literacy, 11, 307–330. doi:10.1177/1468798411409297
  • Vogel, A. C., Miezin, F. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2012). The putative visual word form area is functionally connected to the dorsal attention network. Cerebral Cortex, 22, 537–549. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr100

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.