Publication Cover
Laterality
Asymmetries of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognition
Volume 20, 2015 - Issue 4
1,693
Views
8
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Native reading direction influences lateral biases in the perception of shape from shading

, , &
Pages 418-433 | Received 03 Mar 2014, Accepted 18 Nov 2014, Published online: 24 Dec 2014

REFERENCES

  • Abed, F. (1991). Cultural influences on visual scanning patterns. Journal of Cross-cultural Psychology, 22, 525–534. doi:10.1177/0022022191224006
  • Andrews, B., Aisenberg, D., d'Avossa, G., & Sapir, A. (2013). Cross-cultural effects on the assumed light source direction: Evidence from English and Hebrew readers. Journal of Vision, 13(13), 1–7. doi:10.1167/13.13.2
  • Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1980). Pseudoneglect: Effects of hemispace on tactile line bisection task. Neuropsychologia, 18, 491–498. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(80)90151-7
  • Chokron, S., & De Agostini, M. (2000). Reading habits influence aesthetic preference. Cognitive Brain Research, 10, 45–49. doi:10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00021-5
  • Chokron, S., & Imbert, M. (1993). Influence of reading habits on line bisection. Cognitive Brain Research, 1, 219–222. doi:10.1016/0926-6410(93)90005-P
  • Corbetta, M., Shulman, G. L., Miezin, F. M., & Petersen, S. E. (1995). Superior parietal cortex activation during spatial attention shifts and visual feature conjunction. Science, 270, 802–805. doi:10.1126/science.270.5237.802
  • Elias, L. J., Bryden, M. P., & Bulman-Fleming, M. B. (1998). Footedness is a better predictor of emotional lateralization than is handedness. Neuropsychologia, 36(1), 37–43. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(97)00107-3
  • Elias, L. J., & Robinson, B. M. (2005). Lateral biases in assumptions of lighting position. Brain and Cognition, 59, 303–305. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.08.021
  • Fagard, J., & Dahmen, R. (2003). The effects of reading-writing direction on the asymmetry of space perception and directional tendencies: A comparison between French and Tunisian children. Laterality, 8(1), 39–52. doi:10.1080/713754473
  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–135. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
  • Ishii, Y., Okubo, M., Nicholls, M. E. R., & Imai, H. (2011). Lateral biases and reading direction: Dissociation between aesthetic preference and line bisection. Brain and Cognition, 75, 242–247. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.12.005
  • Kleffner, D. A., & Ramachandran, V. S. (1992). On the perception of shape from shading. Perception & Psychophysics, 52(1), 18–36. doi:10.3758/BF03206757
  • Labar, M. (1973). Turning the left cheek examined using modern photography. Nature, 245, 338. doi:10.1038/245338a0
  • Mamassian, P., & Goutcher, R. (2001). Prior knowledge on the illumination position. Cognition, 81, B1–B9. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00116-0
  • McCourt, M. E. (2001). Performance consistency of normal observers in forced-choice tachistoscopic visual line bisection. Neuropsychologia, 39, 1065–1076. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00044-6
  • McDine, D. A., Livingston, I. J., Thomas, N. A., & Elias, L. J. (2011). Lateral biases in lighting of abstract artwork. Laterality, 16, 268–279. doi:10.1080/13576500903548382
  • McManus, C. I., Buckman, J., & Woolley, E. (2004). Is light in pictures presumed to come from the left side? Perception, 33, 1421–1436. doi:10.1068/p5289
  • Morikawa, K., & McBeath, M. K. (1992). Lateral motion bias associated with reading direction. Vision Research, 32, 1137–1141. doi:10.1016/0042-6989(92)90014-A
  • Nazir, T. A., Ben-Boutayab, N., Decoppet, N., Deutsch, A., & Frost, R. (2004). Reading habits, perceptual learning, and recognition of printed words. Brain and Language, 88, 294–311. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00168-8
  • Nicholls, M. E. R., Bradshaw, J. L., & Mattingley, J. B. (1999). Free-viewing perceptual asymmetries for the judgment of brightness, numerosity and size. Neuropsychologia, 37, 307–314. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00074-8
  • Nicholls, M. E. R., & Roberts, G. R. (2002). Can free-viewing perceptual asymmetries be explained by scanning, pre-motor or attentional biases? Cortex, 38, 113–136. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70645-2
  • Pollatsek, A., Bolozky, S., Well, A. D., & Rayner, K. (1981). Asymmetries in the perceptual span for Israeli Readers. Brain and Language, 14, 174–180. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(81)90073-0
  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.13.1.25
  • Ramachandran, V. S. (1988). Perception of shape from shading. Nature, 331, 163–166. doi:10.1038/331163a0
  • Rinaldi, L., Di Luca, S., Henik, A., & Girelli, L. (2014). Reading direction shifts visuospatial attention: An Interactive Account of attentional biases. Acta Psychologia, 115, 98–105. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.05.018
  • Smith, A. K., & Elias, L. J. (2013). Native reading direction and corresponding preferences for left or right lit images. Perceptual & Motor Skills: Learning & Memory, 116, 355–367. doi:10.2466/23.24.PMS.116.2.355-367
  • Sun, J., & Perona, P. (1998). Where is the sun? Nature, 1, 183–184. doi:10.1038/630
  • Thomas, N. A., Burkitt, J. A., Patrick, R. E., & Elias, L. J. (2008). The lighter side of advertising: Investigating posing and lighting biases. Laterality, 13, 504–513. doi:10.1080/13576500802249538