Publication Cover
Laterality
Asymmetries of Brain, Behaviour, and Cognition
Volume 26, 2021 - Issue 5
287
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Right visual field advantage for lexical decision dependent on stimulus size and visibility: Evidence for an early processing account of hemispheric asymmetry

ORCID Icon
Pages 539-563 | Received 08 Sep 2020, Accepted 23 Nov 2020, Published online: 09 Dec 2020

References

  • Appelbaum, L. G., Liotti, M., Perez, R., III, Fox, S. P., & Woldorff, M.G. (2009). The temporal dynamics of implicit processing of non-letter, letter, and word-forms in the human visual cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 3, 1-11.
  • Batt, V., Underwood, G., & Bryden, M. P. (1995). Inspecting asymmetric presentations of words differing in informational and morphemic structure. Brain and Language, 49(3), 202–223.
  • Beeman, M. J., & Chiarello, C. (1998). Complementary right- and left-hemisphere language comprehension. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7(1), 2–8.
  • Bourne, V. J. (2006). The divided visual field paradigm: Methodological considerations. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 11(4), 373–393.
  • Bradshaw, G. J., Hicks, R. E., & Rose, B. (1979). Lexical discrimination and letter-string identification in the two visual fields. Brain and Language, 8, 10–18.
  • Braun, M., Hutzler, F., Ziegler, J. C., Dambacher, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2009). Pseudohomophone effects provide evidence of early lexico-phonological processing in visual word recognition. Human Brain Mapping, 30(7), 1977–1989.
  • Briggs, G. G., & Kinsbourne, M. (1972). Visual persistence as measured by reaction time. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 24(3), 318–325.
  • Bryden, M. (1982). Laterality: Functional asymmetry in the intact brain. New York: Academic Press.
  • Brysbaert, M. (1994). Interhemispheric transfer and the processing of foveally presented stimuli. Behavioural Brain Research, 64(1–2), 151–161.
  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 41(4), 977–990.
  • Brysbaert, M., Vitu, F., & Schroyens, W. (1996). The right visual field advantage and the optimal viewing position effect: On the relation between foveal and parafoveal word recognition. Neuropsychology, 10(3), 385–395.
  • Chiarello, C., Senehi, J., & Soulier, M. (1986). Viewing conditions and hemisphere asymmetry for the lexical decision. Neuropsychologia, 24(4), 521–529.
  • Christman, S. (1989). Perceptual characteristics in visual laterality research. Brain and Cognition, 11(2), 238–257.
  • Chu, R., Joordens, S., & Meltzer, J. A. (2020). Interhemispheric transfer of semantic information facilitates bilateral word recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 149(5), 984–1005.
  • Clark, H. H. (1973). The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language statistics in psychological research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 12(4), 335–359.
  • Coltheart, M. (1981). The MRC psycholinguistic database. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 33, 497–505.
  • Corballis, P. M., Frendrich, R., Shapley, R., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1999). Illusory contours and amodal completion: Evidence for a functional dissociation in callosotomy patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11, 459–466.
  • Corballis, P. M., Funnell, M. G., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1999). A dissociation between spatial and identity matching in callosotomy patients. Neuroreport, 10, 2183–2187.
  • Corballis, P. M., Funnell, M. G., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetries for simple visual judgments in the split brain. Neuropsychologia, 40(4), 401–410.
  • Corballis, M. C., & Sergent, J. (1988). Imagery in a commissurotomized patient. Neuropsychologia, 26, 13–26.
  • Deason, R. G., & Marsolek, C. J. (2005). A critical boundary to the left-hemisphere advantage in visual-word processing. Brain and Language, 92(3), 251–261.
  • DiCarlo, J. J., Zoccolan, D., & Rust, N. C. (2012). How does the brain solve visual object recognition?. Neuron, 73(3), 415–434.
  • Dickson, D. S., & Federmeier, K. D. (2014). Hemispheric differences in orthographic and semantic processing as revealed by event-related potentials. Neuropsychologia, 64, 230–239.
  • Di Lollo, V. (1980). Temporal integration in visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(1), 75–97.
  • Di Lollo, V., Enns, J. T., Yantis, S., & Dechief, L. D. (2000). Response latencies to the onset and offset of visual stimuli. Perception & Psychophysics, 62, 218–225.
  • Ellis, A. W. (2004). Length, formats, neighbors, hemispheres, and the processing of words presented laterally or at fixation. Brain and Language, 88, 355–366.
  • Ellis, A. W., Ansorge, L., & Lavidor, M. (2007). Words, hemispheres, and dissociable subsystems: The effects of exposure duration, case alternation, priming, and continuity of form on word recognition in the left and right visual fields. Brain and Language, 103(3), 292–303.
  • Forster, B. A., Corballis, P. M., & Corballis, M. C. (2000). Effect of luminance on successiveness discrimination in the absence of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia, 38, 441–450.
  • Funnell, M. G., Corballis, P. M., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1999). A deficit in perceptual matching in the left hemisphere of a callosotomy patient. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1143–1154.
  • Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the corpus callosum enable the human condition? Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 123(7), 1293–1326.
  • Gazzaniga, M. S., & Smylie, C. S. (1983). Facial recognition and brain asymmetries: Clues to underlying mechanisms. Annals of Neurology, 13, 536–540.
  • Gilad-Gutnick, S., Yovel, G., & Sinha, P. (2012). Recognizing degraded faces: The contribution of configural and featural cues. Perception, 41(12), 1497–1511.
  • Gill, K. M., & McKeever, W. F. (1974). Word length and exposure time effects on the recognition of bilaterally presented words. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 4(3), 173–175.
  • Gomez, P., & Perea, M. (2014). Decomposing encoding and decisional components in visual- word recognition: A diffusion model analysis. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(12), 2455–2466.
  • Gomez, P., Ratcliff, R., & Perea, M. (2007). A model of the go/no-go task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(3), 389–413.
  • Hellige, J. B. (1989). Visual laterality and hemisphere specialization: Methodological and theoretical considerations.. In J. B. Sidowski (Ed.), Conditioning, cognition, and methodology: Contemporary issues in experimental psychology (pp. 153–173). New York: University Press.
  • Iaccino, J. (1993). Left brain-right brain differences: Inquiries, evidence, and new approaches. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Jordan, T. R., Patching, G. R., & Milner, A. D. (1998). Central fixations are inadequately controlled by instructions alone: Implications for studying cerebral asymmetry. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 51A(2), 371–391.
  • Jordan, T. R., Patching, G. R., & Milner, A. D. (2000). Lateralized word recognition: Assessing the role of hemispheric specialization, modes of lexical access, and perceptual asymmetry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(3), 1192–1208.
  • Lindell, A. K., Arend, I., Ward, R., Norton, J., & Wathan, J. (2007). Hemispheric asymmetries in feature integration during visual word recognition. Laterality: Asymmetries of Body, Brain and Cognition, 12(6), 543–558.
  • Lindell, A. K., & Nicholls, M. E. R. (2003). Cortical representation of the fovea: Implications for visual half-field research. Cortex, 39, 111–117.
  • MacKavey, W., Curcio, F., & Rosen, J. (1975). Tachistoscopic word recognition performance under conditions of simultaneous bilateral presentation. Neuropsychologia, 13(1), 27–33.
  • Marsolek, C. J., Kosslyn, S. M., & Squire, L. R. (1992). Form-specific visual priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(3), 492–508.
  • Mohr, B., Pulvermüller, F., & Zaidel, E. (1994). Lexical decision after left, right and bilateral presentation of function words, content words and non-words: Evidence for interhemispheric interaction. Neuropsychologia, 32(1), 105–124.
  • Monaghan, P., Shillcock, R., & McDonald, S. (2004). Hemispheric asymmetries in the split- fovea model of semantic processing. Brain and Language, 88(3), 339–354.
  • Moscovitch, M. (1979). Information processing in the cerebral hemispheres. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Handbook of Behavioural Neurobiology. Vol. 2. Neuropsychology (pp. 379–446). New York: Plenum.
  • Nemrodov, D., Harpaz, Y., Javitt, D. C., & Lavidor, M. (2011). ERP evidence of hemispheric independence in visual word recognition. Brain and Language, 118(3), 72–80.
  • Nobre, A. C., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (1994). Word recognition in the human inferior termporal lobe. Nature, 372(6503), 260–263.
  • Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia, 9(1), 97–113.
  • Pegna, A. J., Khateb, A., Michel, C. M., & Landis, T. (2004). Visual recognition of faces, objects, and words using degraded stimuli: Where and when it occurs. Human Brain Mapping, 22(4), 300–311.
  • Perea, M., Gómez, P., & Fraga, I. (2010). Masked nonword repetition effects in yes/no and go/no-go lexical decision: A test of the evidence accumulation and deadline accounts. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(3), 369–374.
  • Pring, T. R. (1981). The effect of stimulus size and exposure duration on visual-field asymmetries. Cortex, 17, 227–239.
  • Racheva, K., & Vassilar, A. (2008). Sensitivity to stimulus onset and offset in the S-cone pathway. Vision Research, 48(9), 1125–1136.
  • Ratcliff, R. (1993). Methods for dealing with reaction time outliers. Psychological Bulletin, 114(3), 510–532.
  • Ratcliff, R. (1981). A theory of order relations in perceptual matching. Psychological Review, 88, 552–572.
  • Ratcliff, R., Gomez, P., & McKoon, G. (2004). A diffusion model account of the lexical decision task. Psychological Review, 111(1), 159–182.
  • Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J. N. (1998). Modeling response times for two-choice decisions. Psychological Science, 9, 347–356.
  • Ratcliff, R., & Rouder, J. N. (2000). A diffusion model account of masking in two-choice letter identification. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26(1), 127–140.
  • Ross, L. E., & Ross, S. M. (1980). Saccade latency and warning signals: Stimulus onset, offset, and change as a warning of events. Perception & Psychophysics, 27, 251–257.
  • Rudell, A. P., & Hua, J. (1995). Recognition potential latency and word image degradation. Brain and Language, 51(2), 229–241.
  • Rutherford, B. J., & Mathesius, J. R. (2012). The brain’s hemispheres and controlled search of the lexicon: Evidence from fixated words and pseudowords. Brain and Cognition, 79(3), 188–199.
  • Sergent, J. (1982a). Influence of luminance on hemispheric processing. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 20(4), 221–223.
  • Sergent, J. (1982b). Theoretical and methodological consequences of variations in exposure duration in visual laterality studies. Perception & Psychophysics, 31(5), 451–461.
  • Sergent, J. (1983). Role of the input in visual hemispheric asymmetries. Psychological Bulletin, 93(3), 481–512.
  • Shillcock, R., Ellison, T. M., & Monaghan, P. (2000). Eye-fixation behavior, lexical storage, and visual word recognition in a split processing model. Psychological Review, 107(4), 824–851.
  • Sinha, P. (2002). Recognizing complex patterns. Nature Neuroscience, 5(Suppl), 1093–1097.
  • Slowiaczek, L. M., & Kahan, T. A. (2014). Evidence for the automatic processing of prelexical codes in an orthographic but not a phonological task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 21(6), 1607–1616.
  • Thorpe, S., Fize, D., & Marlot, C. (1996). Speed of processing in the human visual system. Nature, 381, 520–522.
  • Van der Haegen, L., Brysbaert, M., & Davis, C. J. (2009). How does interhemispheric communication in visual word recognition work? Deciding between early and late integration accounts of the split fovea theory. Brain and Language, 108(2), 112–121.
  • Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 47A(3), 631–650.
  • Weems, S. A., & Zaidel, E.. (2005). The effect of response mode on lateralized lexical decision performance. Neuropsychologia, 43(3), 386–395.
  • Whitney, C. (2001). How the brain encodes the order of letters in a printed word: The SERIOL model and selective literature review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 8(2), 221–243.
  • Wyatte, D., Curran, T., & O’Reilly, R. (2012). The limits of feedforward vision: Recurrent processing promotes robust object recognition when objects are degraded. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24(11), 2248–2261.
  • Young, A. W. (1982). Methodological theoretical bases. In J. G. Beaumont (Ed.), Divided visual field studies of cerebral organisation (pp. 11–27). London: Academic Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.