212
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

How personal experiences shaped risk judgments during COVID-19

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 438-457 | Received 18 Sep 2023, Accepted 25 Apr 2024, Published online: 18 Jun 2024

References

  • Akerlof, K., E. W. Maibach, D. Fitzgerald, A. Y. Cedeno, and A. Neuman. 2013. “Do People “Personally Experience” Global Warming, and If so How, and Does It Matter?” Global Environmental Change 23 (1): 81–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2012.07.006.
  • Broomell, S. B., D. V. Budescu, and H.-H. Por. 2015. “Personal Experience with Climate Change Predicts Intentions to Act.” Global Environmental Change 32: 67–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.gloenvcha.2015.03.001.
  • Bundesinstitut für Bevölkerungsforschung. 2023. Durchschnittsalter der Bevölkerung in Deutschland [Average age of the population of Germany]. https://www.bib.bund.de/Permalink.html?cms_permaid=1217910.
  • Daoust, J.-F. 2020. “Elderly People and Responses to COVID-19 in 27 Countries.” PLoS One 15 (7): E 0235590. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0235590.
  • Demski, C., S. Capstick, N. Pidgeon, R. G. Sposato, and A. Spence. 2017. “Experience of Extreme Weather Affects Climate Change Mitigation and Adaptation Responses.” Climatic Change 140 (2): 149–164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10584-016-1837-4.
  • Denrell, J., and J. G. March. 2001. “Adaptation as Information Restriction: The Hot Stove Effect.” Organization Science 12 (5): 523–538. https://doi.org/10.1287/orsc.12.5.523.10092.
  • Douglas, M. 2013. Risk and Blame. New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Douglas, M., and A. Wildavsky. 1983. Risk and Culture: An Essay on the Selection of Technological and Environmental Dangers. Berkley, CA: University of California Press.
  • Dryhurst, S., C. R. Schneider, J. Kerr, A. L. J. Freeman, G. Recchia, A. M. van der Bles, D. Spiegelhalter, and S. van der Linden. 2020. “Risk Perceptions of COVID-19 around the World.” Journal of Risk Research 23 (7-8): 994–1006. https://doi.org/10.1080/13669877.2020.1758193.
  • Heading, S., and S. Zahidi. 2023. Global Risks Report 2023. Geneva, Switzerland: World Economic Forum.
  • Hertwig, R., R. M. Hogarth, and T. Lejarraga. 2018. “Experience and Description: Exploring Two Paths to Knowledge.” Current Directions in Psychological Science 27 (2): 123–128. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417740645.
  • Hertwig, R., and D. U. Wulff. 2022. “A Description-Experience Framework of the Psychology of Risk.” Perspectives on Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science 17 (3): 631–651. https://doi.org/10.1177/17456916211026896.
  • Kasperson, R. E., O. Renn, P. Slovic, H. S. Brown, J. Emel, R. Goble, J. X. Kasperson, and S. Ratick. 1988. “The Social Amplification of Risk: A Conceptual Framework.” Risk Analysis 8 (2): 177–187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.1988.tb01168.x.
  • Kaufmann, C., M. Weber, and E. Haisley. 2013. “The Role of Experience Sampling and Graphical Displays on One’s Investment Risk Appetite.” Management Science 59 (2): 323–340. https://doi.org/10.1287/mnsc.1120.1607.
  • Kubin, E., C. Puryear, C. Schein, and K. Gray. 2021. “Personal Experiences Bridge Moral and Political Divides Better Than Facts.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 118 (6): E 2008389118. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.2008389118.
  • Luhmann, N. 2004. Ökologische Kommunikation [Ecological Communication]. 4th ed. Wiesbaden, Germany: Springer.
  • Malmendier, U., and S. Nagel. 2011. “Depression Babies: Do Macroeconomic Experiences Affect Risk Taking?” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 126 (1): 373–416. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjq004.
  • Malmendier, U., and S. Nagel. 2016. “Learning from Inflation Experiences.” The Quarterly Journal of Economics 131 (1): 53–87. https://doi.org/10.1093/qje/qjv037.
  • March, J. G. 2010. The Ambiguities of Experience. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
  • Moussaïd, M., H. Brighton, and W. Gaissmaier. 2015. “The Amplification of Risk in Experimental Diffusion Chains.” Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 112 (18): 5631–5636. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1421883112.
  • Myers, T. A., E. W. Maibach, C. Roser-Renouf, K. Akerlof, and A. A. Leiserowitz. 2013. “The Relationship between Personal Experience and Belief in the Reality of Global Warming.” Nature Climate Change 3 (4): 343–347. https://doi.org/10.1038/nclimate1754.
  • Petherick, A., R. Goldszmidt, E. B. Andrade, R. Furst, T. Hale, A. Pott, and A. Wood. 2021. “A Worldwide Assessment of Changes in Adherence to COVID-19 Protective Behaviours and Hypothesized Pandemic Fatigue.” Nature Human Behaviour 5 (9): 1145–1160. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-021-01181-x.
  • Remus, R., U. Quasthoff, and G. Heyer. 2010. “SentiWS - A Publicly Available German-Language Resource for Sentiment Analysis.” In Proceedings of the Seventh International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (LREC’10), edited by N. Calzolari, K. Choukri, B. Maegaard, J. Mariani, J. Odijk, S. Piperidis, M. Rosner, and D. Tapias,1168–1171. Valletta, Malta: European Language Resources Association (ELRA).
  • Renn, O., W. J. Burns, J. X. Kasperson, R. E. Kasperson, and P. Slovic. 1992. “The Social Amplification of Risk: Theoretical Foundations and Empirical Applications.” Journal of Social Issues 48 (4): 137–160. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.1992.tb01949.x.
  • Reser, J. P., G. L. Bradley, and M. C. Ellul. 2014. “Encountering Climate Change: ‘Seeing’ Is More than ‘Believing.” WIREs Climate Change 5 (4): 521–537. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcc.286.
  • Shahid, Z., R. Kalayanamitra, B. McClafferty, D. Kepko, D. Ramgobin, R. Patel, C. S. Aggarwal, et al. 2020. “COVID-19 and Older Adults: What We Know.” Journal of the American Geriatrics Society 68 (5): 926–929. https://doi.org/10.1111/jgs.16472.
  • Slovic, P. 1987. “Perception of Risk.” Science 236 (4799): 280–285. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.3563507.
  • Stone, L., R. Olinky, and A. Huppert. 2007. “Seasonal Dynamics of Recurrent Epidemics.” Nature 446 (7135): 533–536. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature05638.
  • Sutton, R. S., and A. G. Barto. 1998. Reinforcement Learning: An Introduction. Adaptive Computation and Machine Learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Trope, Y., and N. Liberman. 2010. “Construal-Level Theory of Psychological Distance.” Psychological Review 117 (2): 440–463. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0018963.
  • Van der Linden, S. 2014. “On the Relationship between Personal Experience, Affect and Risk Perception: The Case of Climate Change.” European Journal of Social Psychology 44 (5): 430–440. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2008.
  • Viscusi, W. K., and R. J. Zeckhauser. 2015. “The Relative Weights of Direct and Indirect Experiences in the Formation of Environmental Risk Beliefs.” Risk Analysis 35 (2): 318–331. https://doi.org/10.1111/risa.12271.
  • Wachinger, G., O. Renn, C. Begg, and C. Kuhlicke. 2013. “The Risk Perception Paradox: Implications for Governance and Communication of Natural Hazards.” Risk Analysis 33 (6): 1049–1065. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1539-6924.2012.01942.x.
  • Wegwarth, O., U. Mansmann, F. Zepp, D. Lühmann, R. Hertwig, and M. Scherer. 2023. “Vaccination Intention following Receipt of Vaccine Information through Interactive Simulation vs Text among COVID-19 Vaccine-Hesitant Adults during the Omicron Wave in Germany.” JAMA Network Open 6 (2): E 2256208. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.56208.
  • Weinstein, N. D. 1982. “Unrealistic Optimism about Susceptibility to Health Problems.” Journal of Behavioral Medicine 5 (4): 441–460. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00845372.