156
Views
27
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Limitations in working memory: implications for language development

Pages 95-116 | Published online: 03 Jul 2009

  • ADAMS, A.-M. and GATHERCOLE, S. E., 1995, Phonological working memory and speech production in pre-school children. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 38, 403-414.
  • ADAMS, A.-M and GATHERCOLE, S. E., 1996, Phonological working memory and spoken language development in young children. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 216-233.
  • BADDELEY, A. D., 1986 Working Memory (Oxford: Oxford University Press).
  • BADDELEY, A. D., 1996. Exploring the central executive. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49A, 5-28.
  • BADDELEY, A. D. and WILSON, B., 1985, Phonological coding and short-term memory in patients without speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 24, 490-502.
  • BADDELEY, A. D., GATHERCOLE, S. E. and PAPAGNO, C., 1998, The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158-173.
  • BADDELEY, A. D., PAPAGNO, C. and VALLAR, G., 1988, When long-term learning depends on short-term storage. Journal of Memory and Language, 27, 586-596.
  • BARNARD, P., 1985, Interacting cognitive subsystems: A psycholinguistic approach for short-term memory. In A. E. Ellis (ed.), Pogress in the Psychology of Language, vol. 2 (Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), pp. 197-258.
  • BATES, E., BRETHERTON, I. and SNYDER, L., 1988, From First Words to Grammar: Individual Differenccs and Dissociable Mechanisms (New York: Cambridge University Press).
  • BISHOP, D. V. M., 1994, Grammatical errors in specific language impairment: Competence or performance limitations? Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 507-550.
  • BISHOP, D. V. M, 1997, Uncommon Understanding: Development and Disorders of Language Comprehension in Children (Hove: Psychology Press).
  • BISHOP, D. V. M, NORTH, T. and DONIAN, C., 1996, Nonword repetition as a behavioural marker for inherited language impairment: evidence from a twin study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 391-404.
  • BLAKE, J., AUSTIN, W, CANNON, M, LISUS, A. and VAUGHAN, A., 1994, The relationship between memory span and measures of imitative and spontaneous language complexity in preschool children. International Journal of Behavioural Development, 17, 91-107.
  • BOYLE, M. K. and GERKEN, L., 1997, The influence of lexical familiarity on children's function morpheme omissions: a nonmetrical effect?. Journal of Memory and Language. 36, 117-128.
  • BROWN, R. A., 1973, A First Language The Early Stages (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press).
  • CARAMATA, S. M. and SCHWARTZ, R. G., 1985, Production of object words and action words: Evidence for a relationship between phonology and semantics. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 28, 323-330.
  • CARPENTER, P. A., MIYAKE, A. and JUST, M. A., 1994, Working memory constraints in comprehension. In M A. Gernsbacher (ed.), Handbook of Psycholinguistics (London: Academic Press), pp. 1075-1122.
  • CARROLL, J. B., DAVIES, P. and RICHMAN, B., 1971, The Word Frequency Book (New York: American Heritage).
  • CRYSTAL, D., 1987, Towards a 'bucket' theory of language disability: taking account of interaction between linguistic levels. Clinical Linguistics and Phonetics, 1, 7-22.
  • DE RENZI, E. and NICHELLI, P., 1975, Verbal and non-verbal short-term memory impairment following hemispheric damage. Cortex, 11, 341-354.
  • DELL, G. S., 1986, A spreading activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Revoir, 93, 283-321.
  • ELLIS WEISMER, S. and HESKETH, L. J., 1993, The influence of prosodie and gestural cues on novel word acquisition by children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 36, 1013-1025.
  • ELLIS WEISMER, S. and HESKETH, L. J., 1996, Lexical learning by children with specific language impairment: Effects of linguistic input presented at varying speaking rates. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 39, 177-190.
  • GATHERCOLE, S. E., 1995, Is nonword repetition a test of phonological memory or lexical knowledge? It all depends on the nonwords. Memory and Cognition, 23, 83-94.
  • GATHERCOLE, S. E. and BADDELEY, A. D., 1990, Phonological memory deficits in language disordered children: Is there a causal connection? Journal of Memory and Language, 29, 336-360.
  • GATHERCOLE, S. E. and BADDELEY, A. D., 1993, Working Memory and Language (Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).
  • GATHERCOLE, S. E. and BADDELEY, A. D., 1996, The Children's Test of Nonword Repetition (Psychological Corp.).
  • GATHERCOLE, S. E. and MARTIN, A. J., 1996, Interactive processes in phonological memory. In S. E. Gathercole (ed.), Models of Short-term Memory (Hove: Psychology Press), pp. 73-100.
  • GERKEN, L., 1991, The metrical basis for children's subjectless sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 431-451.
  • GILLAM, R. B. and VAN KLEECK, A., 1996, Phonological awareness training and short-term working memory: clinical implications. Topics in Language Disorders, 17, 72-81.
  • GOPNIK, M and CRAGO, M., 1991, Familial aggregation of a developmental language disorder. Cognition, 39, 1-50.
  • HOWARD, D. and VAN DER LELY, H. K. J., 1995, Specific Language impairment in children is not due to a short-term memory deficit. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 38, 466-472.
  • HULME, C., MAUGHAN, S. and BROWN, G. D. A., 1991, Memory for familiar and unfamiliar words: Evidence for a long-term memory contribution to short-term memory span. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 685-701.
  • ISAACS, E. B. and VARGHA-KHADEM, F., 1989, Differential course of development of spatial and verbal memory span: a normative study. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 377-380.
  • JUST, M. A. and CARPENTER, P. A., 1992, A capacity theory of comprehension: Individual differences in working memory. Psychological Review, 99, 122-149.
  • LAHEY, M. and BLOOM, L., 1994, Variability and language learning disabilities. In G. Wallach and K. Butler (eds), Language Learning Disabilities in School-age Children and Adolescents (New York: Macmillan College), pp. 355-372.
  • LAHEY, M, LIEBERGOTT, J., CHESNICK, MENYUK, P. and ADAMS, J., 1992, Variability in the children's use of grammatical morphemes. Applied Psycholinguistics, 13, 373-398.
  • LEONARD, L. B., 1991, Specific language impairment as a clinical category. Language, Speech and Heating Services in the Schools, 22, 66-68.
  • LEVELT, W. J. M, 1989, Speaking: From Intention to Articulation (Cambridge, MA: MIT Press).
  • MILLER, J. F., 1981, Assessing Language Production in Child ten: Experimental Procedures (Baltimore: University Park Press).
  • MILLER, J. F. and CHAPMAN, R. S., 1981, The relation between age and mean length of utterance in morphemes. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 24, 154-161.
  • MILLER, J. F. and CHAPMAN, R., 1992, Systematic Analysis of Language Transcripts for the Apple Macintosh Computer (Madison: Language Analysis Laboratory, Waisman Centre on Mental Retardation and Human Development).
  • MONSELL, S., 1984, Components of working memory underlying verbal skills: a 'distributed capacities' view. In H. Bouma and D. G. Bouwhuis (eds), Attention and Performance X: Control of Language Processes (Hove: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates), pp. 327-350.
  • MONTGOMERY, J. W, 1995, Examination of phonological working memory in specifically language-impaired children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 16, 355-378.
  • NELSON, L. K. and BAUER, H. R., 1991, Speech and language production at age 2: Evidence for trade-offs between linguistic and phonetic processing. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 34, 879-892.
  • OHALA, D. and GERKEN, L., 1997, Lexical familiarity effects on children's weak syllable omissions. In Proceedings of the 21st Boston University Conference on Language Development, Boston, MA (Cascadilla Press).
  • PANAGOS, J. and PRELOCK, P., 1982, Phonological constraints on the sentence production of language-disordered children. Journal of Speech and Heating Research, 25, 171-177.
  • QUINLAN, P. T., 1992, The MRC Psycholinguistic Database (London: Oxford University Press).
  • RAVEN, J., 1977, Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices (London: H. K. Lewis).
  • SCARBOROUGH, H. S., 1990, Index of productive syntax. Applied Psycholinguistics, 11, 1-22.
  • SPEIDEL, G. E., 1989a, Imitation: a bootstrap for learning to speak? In G. E. Speidel and K. E. Nelson (eds), The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning (New York: Springer), pp. 151-179.
  • SPEIDEL, G. E., 1989b, A biological basis for individual differences in learning to speak. In G. E. Speidel and K. E. Nelson (eds), The Mary Faces of Imitation in Language Learning (New York: Springer), pp. 199-229
  • SPEIDEL, G. E., 1993, Phonological short-term memory and individual differences in learning to speak: a bilingual case study. First Language, 13, 69-91.
  • SPEIDEL, G. E. and HERRESHOFF, M. J., 1989, Imitation and the construction of long utterances. In G. E. Speidel and K. E. Nelson (eds), The Many Faces of Imitation in Language Learning (New York: Springer), pp. 181-197.
  • STREIM, N. W. and CHAPMAN, R. S., 1987, The effects of discourse support on the organisation and production of children's utterances. Applied Pycholinguistics, 8, 55-66.
  • TALLAL, P., MILLER, S. and FITCH, R. H., 1993, Neurobiological basis of speech: a case for the preeminence of temporal processing. In P. Tallal, A. M Galaburda, R. R. Llinas and C. von Euler (eds), Annals of The New York Academy Of Stierte: Temporal Information Processing in the Nervous System (New York: NYAS).
  • TURNER, M. L. and ENGLE, R. W., 1989, Is working memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory aid Language, 28, 127-154.
  • VALIAN, V., 1991, Syntactic subjects in the early speech of American and Italian children. Cognition, 40, 21-81.
  • VAN DER LELY, H. K. J., 1994, Canonical linking rules forward versus reverse linking in normally developing and specifically language-impaired children. Cognition, 51, 29-72.
  • VAN DER LELY, H. K. J. and HOWARD, D., 1993, Children with specific language impairment: linguistic impairment or short-term memory deficit? Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 36, 1193-1207.
  • WATERS, G. S., ROCHON, E. and CAPLAN, D., 1992, The role of high-level planning in rehearsal: Evidence from patients with apraxia of speech. Journal of Memory and Language, 31, 54-73.
  • WILSON, J. T. L., SCOTT, J. H. and POWER, K. G., 1987, Developmental differences in the span of visual memory for pattern. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 249-255.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.