Publication Cover
Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition
A Journal on Normal and Dysfunctional Development
Volume 28, 2021 - Issue 1
475
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Article

Age differences in neural activity related to mentalizing during person perception

ORCID Icon, &
Pages 143-160 | Received 05 Sep 2019, Accepted 13 Jan 2020, Published online: 21 Jan 2020

References

  • Adams, R., Rule, N., Franklin, R., Jr., Wang, E., Stevenson, M., Yoshikawa, S., … Ambady, N. (2009). Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: An fMRI investigation. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(1), 97–108.
  • Amodio, D. (2014). The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(10), 670–682.
  • Amodio, D., & Frith, C. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7, 268–277.
  • Bailey, P., Henry, J., & von Hippel, W. (2008). Empathy and social functioning in late adulthood. Aging & Mental Health, 12(4), 499–503.
  • Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Hill, J., Raste, Y., & Plumb, I. (2001). The “reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42(2), 241–251.
  • Cassidy, B., & Krendl, A. (2016). Dynamic neural mechanisms underlie race disparities in social cognition. NeuroImage, 132, 238–246.
  • Cassidy, B., Krendl, A., Stanko, K., Rydell, R., Young, S., & Hugenberg, K. (2017). Configural face processing impacts race disparities in humanization and trust. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 73, 111–124.
  • Cassidy, B., Lee, E., & Krendl, A. (2016). Age and executive ability impact the neural correlates of race perception. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(11), 1752–1761.
  • Cassidy, B., Leshikar, E., Shih, J., Aizenman, A., & Gutchess, A. (2013). Valence-based age differences in medial prefrontal activity during impression formation. Social Neuroscience, 8(5), 462–473.
  • Cornwell, E., & Waite, L. (2009). Social disconnectedness, perceived isolation, and health among older adults. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 50(1), 31–48.
  • Cunningham, W., Johnson, M., Raye, C., Gatenby, J., Gore, J., & Banaji, M. (2004). Separable neural components in the processing of black and white faces. Psychological Science, 15(12), 806–813.
  • Declerck, C.H., & Bogaert, S. (2008). Social value orientation: Related to empathy and the ability to read the mind in the eyes. The Journal of Social Psychology, 148(6), 711–726.
  • Dungan, J., Stepanovic, M., & Young, L. (2016). Theory of mind for processing unexpected events across contexts. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(8), 1183–1192.
  • Folstein, M., Folstein, S., & McHugh, P. (1975). Mini-mental state: A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Jounral of Psychiatric Research, 12(3), 189–198.
  • Franklin, R., Jr, & Zebrowitz, L. (2016). Aging-related changes in decoding negative complex mental states from faces. Experimental Aging Research, 42(5), 471–478.
  • Franklin, R., Jr., & Zebrowitz, L. (2017). Age differences in emotion recognition: Task demands or perceptual dedifferentiation? Experimental Aging Research, 43(5), 453–466.
  • Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531–534.
  • Galinsky, A., & Todd, A. (2014). Perspective-taking as a strategy for improving intergroup relations: Evidence, mechanisms, and qualifications. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 8, 374–387.
  • Gallagher, H., & Frith, C. (2003). Functional imaging of “theory of mind”. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(2), 77–82.
  • Gao, W., Cao, B., Shan, S., Chen, X., Zhou, D., Zhang, X., & Zhao, D. (2008). The CAS-PEAL large-scale Chinese face database and baseline evaluations. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics Part A: Systems and Humans, 38(1), 149–161.
  • Goh, J., Zuzuki, A., & Park, D. (2010). Reduced neural selectivity increases FMRI adaptation with age during face discrimination. NeuroImage, 51, 336–344.
  • Gonsalkorale, K., Sherman, J., & Klauer, K. (2009). Aging and prejudice: Diminished regulation of automatic race bias among older adults. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 410–414.
  • Gottlich, M., Ye, Z., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., Munte, T., & Kramer, U. (2017). Viewing socio-affective stimuli increases connectivity within an extended default mode network. NeuroImage, 148, 8–19.
  • Harkness, K., Sabbagh, M., Jacobson, J., Chowdrey, N., & Chen, T. (2005). Enhanced accuracy of mental state decoding in dysphoric college students. Cognition & Emotion, 19(7), 999–1025.
  • Harris, L., & Fiske, S. (2006). Dehumanizing the lowest of the low: Neuroimaging responses to extreme outgroups. Psychological Science, 17, 847–853.
  • Harris, L., & Fiske, S. (2007). Social groups that elicit disgust are differentially processed in mPFC. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 45–51.
  • Hayes, A. (2012). PROCESS: A versatile computational tool for observed variable mediation, moderation, and conditional process modeling. Retrieved from http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf
  • Hedden, T., Park, D., Nisbett, R., Ji, L., Jing, Q., & Jiao, S. (2002). Cultural variation in verbal versus spatial neuropsychological function across the lifespan. Neuropsychology, 16, 65–73.
  • Henry, J., Phillips, L., Ruffman, T., & Bailey, P. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and Aging, 28(3), 826–839.
  • Hughes, C., Cassidy, B., Faskowitz, J., Avena-Koenigsberger, A., Sporns, O., & Krendl, A. (2019). Changes in neural connections within the default mode network underlie age-related deficits in theory of mind. NeuroImage, 191, 269–277.
  • Jack, A., Dawson, A., & Norr, M. (2013). Seeing human: Distinct and overlapping neural signatures associated with two forms of dehumanization. NeuroImage, 79, 313–328.
  • Kringelbach, M., & Rolls, E. (2003). Neural correlates of rapid reversal learning in a simple model of human social interaction. NeuroImage, 20, 1371–1383.
  • Laillier, R., Viard, A., Caillaud, M., Duclos, H., Bejanin, A., de La Sayette, V., … Laisney, M. (2019). Neurocognitive determinants of theory of mind across the adult lifespan. Brain and Cognition, 136, 103588.
  • Lee, S., Wong, N., & Alvarez, A. (2009). The model minority and the perpetual foreigner: Stereotypes of Asian Americans. In N. Tewari & A. Alvarez (Eds.), Asian American Psychology: Current perspectives (pp. 69–84). New York, NY: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group.
  • Lee, T., Ip, A., Wang, K., Xi, C., Hu, P., Mak, H., … Chan, C. (2010). Faux pas deficits in people with medial prefrontal lesions as related to impaired understanding of a speaker’s mental state. Neuropsychologia, 48(6), 1670–1676.
  • Leslie, A. (1987). Pretense and representation: The origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94(4), 412–426.
  • Li, T., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Correll, J., & Cloutier, J. (2016). The impact of motivation on race-based impression formation. NeuroImage, 124, 1–7.
  • Ma, N., Vandekerckhove, M., Van Overwalle, F., Seurinck, R., & Fias, W. (2011). Spontaneous and intentional trait inferences recruit a common mentalizing network to a different degree: Spontaneous inferences activate only its core areas. Social Neuroscience, 6(2), 123–138.
  • Mars, R., Neubert, F., Noonan, M., Sallet, J., Toni, I., & Rushworth, M. (2012). On the relationship between the “default mode network” and the “social brain”. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00189
  • Mathur, V., Harada, T., & Chiao, J. (2011). Racial identification modulates default network activity for same and other races. Human Brain Mapping, 33(8), 1883–1893.
  • Messick, D., & Mackie, D. (1989). Intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 40, 45–81.
  • Moran, J. (2013). Lifespan development: The effects of typical aging on theory of mind. Behavioural Brain Research, 237, 32–40.
  • Moran, J., Jolly, E., & Mitchell, J. (2012). Social-cognitive deficits in normal aging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 32(16), 5553–5561.
  • Ng, S.Y., Zebrowitz, L.A., & Franklin, Jr, R.G. (2014). Age differences in the differentiation of trait impressions from faces. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 71(2), 220–229.
  • Oswald, F., McAbee, S., Redick, T., & Hambrick, D. (2015). The development of a short domain-general measure of working memory capacity. Behavior Research Methods, 47(4), 1343–1355.
  • Park, D., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T., Davidson, N., Smith, A., & Smith, P. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17(2), 299–320.
  • Pettigrew, T., & Tropp, L. (2008). How does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Meta-analytic tests of three mediators. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(6), 922–934.
  • Poldrack, R. (2007). Region of interest analysis for fMRI. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(1), 67–70.
  • Powers, K., Chavez, R., & Heatherton, T. (2015). Individual differences in response of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex predict daily social behavior. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(1), 121–126.
  • Richardson, C., Bucks, R., & Hogan, A. (2011). Effects of aging on habituation to novelty: An ERP study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 79(2), 97–105.
  • Salat, D., Buckner, R., Snyder, A., Greve, D., Deskikan, R., Busa, E., … Fischl, B. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 14(7), 721–730.
  • Samanez-Larkin, G., Wagner, A., & Knutson, B. (2011). Expected value information improves financial risk taking across the adult life span. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(2), 207–217.
  • Saxe, R., & Kanwisher, N. (2003). People thinking about thinking people: The role of the temporo-parietal junction in “theory of mind”. NeuroImage, 19(4), 1835–1842.
  • Schilbach, L., Eickhoff, S., Rotarska-Jagiela, A., Fink, G., & Vogeley, K. (2008). Minds at rest? Social cognition as the default mode of cognizing and its putative relationship to the “default system” of the brain. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 457–467.
  • Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 9–34.
  • Shih, M., Wang, E., Bucher, A., & Stotzer, R. (2009). Perspective taking: Reducing prejudice toward general outgroups and specific individuals. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 12(5), 565–577.
  • Shipley, W. (1986). Shipley Institute of Living Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.
  • Slessor, G., Phillips, L., & Bull, R. (2007). Exploring the specificity of age-related differences in theory of mind tasks. Psychology and Aging, 22(3), 639–643.
  • Spiers, H., & Maguire, E. (2006). Spontaneous mentalizing during an interactive real world task: An fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 44(10), 1674–1682.
  • Suzuki, A., Ueno, M., Ishikawa, K., Kobayashi, A., Okubo, M., & Nakai, T. (2019). Age-related differences in the activation of the mentalizing- and reward-related brain regions durign the learning of others’ true trustworthiness. Neurobiology of Aging, 73, 1–8.
  • Taylor, C., Lee, J., & Stern, B. (1995). Portrayals of African, Hispanic, and Asian Americans in magazine advertising. American Behavioral Scientist, 38(4), 608–621.
  • Telzer, E., Masten, C., Berkman, E., Lieberman, M., & Fuligni, A. (2011). Neural regions associated with self control and mentalizing are recruited during prosocial behaviors towards the family. NeuroImage, 58(1), 242–249.
  • Todd, A., Bodenhausen, G., Richeson, J., & Galinsky, A. (2011). Perspective taking combats automatic expressions of racial bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(6), 1027–1042.
  • Todorov, A., Said, C., Engell, A., & Oosterhof, N. (2008). Understanding evaluation of faces on social dimensions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(12), 455–460.
  • Van Overwalle, F. (2009). Social cognition and the brain: A meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 30(3), 829–858.
  • Van Overwalle, F., & Vandekerckhove, M. (2013). Implicit and explicit social mentalizing: Dual processes driven by a shared neural network. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 560.
  • Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X., & Han, S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. The Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 8525–8529.
  • Young, L., Camprodon, J., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimululation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(15), 6753–6758.
  • Zaitchik, D. (1990). When representations conflict with reality: The preschooler’s problem with false beliefs and “false” photographs. Cognition, 35(1), 41–68.
  • Zebrowitz, L., Ward, N., Boshyan, J., Gutchess, A., & Hadjikhani, N. (2016). Dedifferentiated face processing in older adults is linked to lower resting state metabolic activity in fusiform face area. Brain Research, 1644, 22–31.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.