322
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Clinical Issues

Intra-individual variability identifies individuals vulnerable to contextually induced executive lapses

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon &
Pages 322-349 | Received 01 Nov 2021, Accepted 15 Mar 2022, Published online: 08 Apr 2022

References

  • Alhola, P., & Polo-Kantola, P. (2007). Sleep deprivation: Impact on cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 3(5), 553–567.
  • Allaire, J. C., & Marsiske, M. (2005). Intraindividual variability may not always indicate vulnerability in elders’ cognitive performance. Psychology and Aging, 20(3), 390–401. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.20.3.390
  • Bauer, D. J., & Curran, P. J. (2005). Probing interactions in fixed and multilevel regression: Inferential and graphical techniques. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 40(3), 373–400. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327906mbr4003_5
  • Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). (1037). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252–1265. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.5.1252 https://doi.org/10 /0022–3514.74.5.1252
  • Bellgrove, M. A., Hester, R., & Garavan, H. (2004). The functional neuroanatomical correlates of response variability: Evidence from a response inhibition task. Neuropsychologia, 42(14), 1910–1916. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.05.007
  • Berg, C. A., Wiebe, D. J., Suchy, Y., Turner, S. L., Butner, J., Munion, A., Lansing, A. H., White, P. C., & Murray, M. (2018). Executive function predicting longitudinal change in Type 1 diabetes management during the transition to emerging adulthood. Diabetes Care, 41(11), 2281–2288. https://doi.org/10.2337/dc18-0351
  • Berryman, C., Stanton, T. R., Bowering, K. J., Tabor, A., McFarlane, A., & Moseley, G. L. (2014). Do people with chronic pain have impaired executive function? A meta-analytical review. Clinical Psychology Review, 34(7), 563–579. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2014.08.003
  • Bielak, A. A. M., Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E., MacDonald, S. W. S., & Hunter, M. A. (2010). Intraindividual variability in reaction time predicts cognitive outcomes 5 years later. Neuropsychology, 24(6), 731–741. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019802
  • Brose, A., Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., & Lindenberger, U. (2012). Daily variability in working memory is coupled with negative affect: The role of attention and motivation. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 12(3), 605–617. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024436
  • Brose, A., Schmiedek, F., Lövdén, M., Molenaar, P., & Lindenberger, U. (2010). Adult age differences in covariation of motivation and working memory performance: Contrasting between-person and within-person findings. Research in Human Development, 7(1), 61–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427600903578177
  • Bunce, D., Anstey, K. J., Christensen, H., Dear, K., Wen, W., & Sachdev, P. (2007). White matter hyperintensities and within-person variability in community-dwelling adults aged 60-64 years. Neuropsychologia, 45(9), 2009–2015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.02.006
  • Caixeta, L., Soares, V. L. D., Vieira, R. T., Soares, C. D., Caixeta, V., Ferreira, S. B., & Aversi-Ferreira, T. A. (2017). Executive function is selectively impaired in old age bipolar depression. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 194. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00194
  • Cheng, Y.-W., Chen, T.-F., & Chiu, M.-J. (2017). From mild cognitive impairment to subjective cognitive decline: Conceptual and methodological evolution. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 13, 491–498. https://doi.org/10.2147/NDT.S123428
  • Cherbuin, N., Sachdev, P., & Anstey, K. J. (2010). Neuropsychological predictors of transition from healthy cognitive aging to mild cognitive impairment: The PATH through life study. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry : Official Journal of the American Association for Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(8), 723–733. https://doi.org/10.1097/jgp.0b013e3181cdecf1
  • Christ, B. U., Combrinck, M. I., & Thomas, K. G. F. (2018). Both reaction time and accuracy measures of intraindividual variability predict cognitive performance in Alzheimer’s Disease. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, 124. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00124
  • Chuah, Y. M. L., Venkatraman, V., Dinges, D. F., & Chee, M. W. L. (2006). The neural basis of interindividual variability in inhibitory Efficiency after sleep deprivation. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 26(27), 7156–7162. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0906-06.2006
  • Clark, L. R., Schiehser, D. M., Weissberger, G. H., Salmon, D. P., Delis, D. C., & Bondi, M. W. (2012). Specific measures of executive function predict cognitive decline in older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society: JINS, 18(1), 118–127. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617711001524
  • Costa, A. S., Dogan, I., Schulz, J. B., & Reetz, K. (2019). Going beyond the mean: Intraindividual variability of cognitive performance in prodromal and early neurodegenerative disorders. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 33(2), 369–389. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2018.1533587
  • Cutuli, D. (2014). Cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression strategies role in the emotion regulation: An overview on their modulatory effects and neural correlates. Frontiers in Systems Neuroscience, 8, 175–180. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2014.00175
  • D’Avanzato, C., Joormann, J., Siemer, M., & Gotlib, I. H. (2013). Emotion regulation in depression and anxiety: Examining diagnostic specificity and stability of strategy use. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 37(5), 968–980. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-013-9537-0
  • Deary, I. J., & Der, G. (2005). Reaction Time, Age, and Cognitive Ability: Longitudinal Findings from Age 16 to 63 Years in Representative Population Samples. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 12(2), 187–215. https://doi.org/10.1080/13825580590969235
  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive function system. Pearson.
  • Dixon, R. A., Garrett, D. D., Lentz, T. L., MacDonald, S. W. S., Strauss, E., & Hultsch, D. F. (2007). Neurocognitive markers of cognitive impairment: Exploring the roles of speed and inconsistency. Neuropsychology, 21(3), 381–399. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.21.3.381
  • Duffy, J. F., Zitting, K.-M., & Chinoy, E. D. (2015). Aging and circadian rhythms. Sleep Medicine Clinics, 10(4), 423–434. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jsmc.2015.08.002
  • Dux, M. C., Woodard, J. L., Calamari, J. E., Messina, M., Arora, S., Chik, H., & Pontarelli, N. (2008). The moderating role of negative affect on objective verbal memory performance and subjective memory complaints in healthy older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 14(2), 327–336. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617708080363
  • Dykiert, D., Der, G., Starr, J. M., & Deary, I. J. (2012). Age differences in intra-individual variability in simple and choice reaction time: Systematic review and meta-analysis. PloS One, 7(10), e45759. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0045759
  • Ehring, T., Tuschen-Caffier, B., Schnülle, J., Fischer, S., & Gross, J. J. (2010). Emotion regulation and vulnerability to depression: Spontaneous versus instructed use of emotion suppression and reappraisal. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 10(4), 563–572. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019010
  • Eraydin, I. E., Mueller, C., Corbett, A., Ballard, C., Brooker, H., Wesnes, K., Aarsland, D., & Huntley, J. (2019). Investigating the relationship between age of onset of depressive disorder and cognitive function. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 34(1), 38–46. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.4979
  • Ettenhofer, M. L., Foley, J., Behdin, N., Levine, A. J., Castellon, S. A., & Hinkin, C. H. (2010). Reaction time variability in HIV-positive individuals. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 25(8), 791–798. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acq064
  • Euler, M. J., Niermeyer, M. A., & Suchy, Y. (2016). Neurocognitive and neurophysiological correlates of motor planning during familiar and novel contexts. Neuropsychology, 30(1), 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000219
  • Fortier-Brochu, É., Beaulieu-Bonneau, S., Ivers, H., & Morin, C. M. (2012). Insomnia and daytime cognitive performance: A meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 16(1), 83–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2011.03.008
  • Franchow, E. I., & Suchy, Y. (2015). Naturally-occurring expressive suppression in daily life depletes executive functioning. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 15(1), 78–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000013
  • Franchow, E. I., & Suchy, Y. (2017). Expressive suppression depletes executive functioning in older adulthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 23(4), 341–351. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000054
  • Garcia-Ptacek, S., Eriksdotter, M., Jelic, V., Porta-Etessam, J., Kåreholt, I., & Manzano Palomo, S. (2016). Subjective cognitive impairment: Towards early identification of Alzheimer disease. Neurología (English Edition), 31(8), 562–571. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nrleng.2013.02.011
  • Goh, J. O. S. (2011). Functional Dedifferentiation and Altered Connectivity in Older Adults: Neural Accounts of Cognitive Aging. Aging and Disease, 2(1), 30–48.
  • Gross, J. J. (1998). Antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation: Divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 224–237. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
  • Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology, 39(3), 281–291. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0048577201393198
  • Gross, J. J., & Levenson, R. W. (1993). Emotional suppression: Physiology, self-report, and expressive behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(6), 970–986. [Database] https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.64.6.970
  • Gunstad, J., Spitznagel, M. B., Luyster, F., Cohen, R. A., & Paul, R. H. (2007). Handedness and cognition across the healthy lifespan. The International Journal of Neuroscience, 117(4), 477–485. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207450600773483
  • Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach (2nd ed.). Guliford Press.
  • Haynes, B. I., Bauermeister, S., & Bunce, D. (2017). A systematic review of longitudinal associations between reaction time intraindividual variability and age-related cognitive decline or impairment, dementia, and mortality. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 23(5), 431–445. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617717000236
  • Hu, T., Zhang, D., Wang, J., Mistry, R., Ran, G., & Wang, X. (2014). Relation between emotion regulation and mental health: A meta-analysis review. Psychological Reports, 114(2), 341–362. https://doi.org/10.2466/03.20.PR0.114k22w4
  • Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., & Dixon, R. A. (2002). Variability in reaction time performance of younger and older adults. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57(2), P101–115. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/57.2.p101
  • Hultsch, D. F., MacDonald, S. W. S., Hunter, M. A., Levy-Bencheton, J., & Strauss, E. (2000). Intraindividual variability in cognitive performance in older adults: Comparison of adults with mild dementia, adults with arthritis, and healthy adults. Neuropsychology, 14(4), 588–598. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.14.4.588
  • Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E. H., & Hunter, M. A. (2011). Intraindividual variability, cognition, and aging. In The handbook of cognitive aging (3rd ed., pp. 491–556). Psychology Press.
  • Inzlicht, M., & Gutsell, J. N. (2007). Running on empty: Neural signals for self-control failure. Psychological Science, 18(11), 933–937. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.02004.x
  • John, O. P., & Gross, J. J. (2004). Healthy and unhealthy emotion regulation: Personality processes, individual differences, and life span development. Journal of Personality, 72(6), 1301–1334. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00298.x
  • Johns, M., Inzlicht, M., & Schmader, T. (2008). Stereotype threat and executive resource depletion: Examining the influence of emotion regulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 137(4), 691–705. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013834
  • Johnson, P. O., & Neyman, J. (1936). Tests of certain linear hypotheses and their application to some educational problems. Statistical Research Memoirs, 1, 57–93.
  • Johnson-Greene, D. (2004). Dementia Rating Scale-2 (DRS-2). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(1), 145–147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.acn.2003.07.003
  • Joormann, J., & Tanovic, E. (2015). Cognitive vulnerability to depression: Examining cognitive control and emotion regulation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 4, 86–92. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.006
  • Jurica, P. J., Leitten, C. L., & Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia Rating Scale–2TM professional manual Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Kane, M. J., Brown, L. H., McVay, J. C., Silvia, P. J., Myin-Germeys, I., & Kwapil, T. R. (2007). For whom the mind wanders, and when: An experience-sampling study of working memory and executive control in daily life. Psychological Science, 18(7), 614–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01948.x
  • Kelly, A. M. C., Uddin, L. Q., Biswal, B. B., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2008). Competition between functional brain networks mediates behavioral variability. NeuroImage, 39(1), 527–537. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.08.008
  • Killgore, W. D. S., Grugle, N. L., Reichardt, R. M., Killgore, D. B., & Balkin, T. J. (2009). Executive functions and the ability to sustain vigilance during sleep loss. Aviation, Space, and Environmental Medicine, 80(2), 81–87. https://doi.org/10.3357/asem.2396.2009
  • Kørner, A., Lauritzen, L., Abelskov, K., Gulmann, N., Marie Brodersen, A., Wedervang-Jensen, T., & Kjeldgaard, M. K. (2006). The Geriatric Depression Scale and the Cornell Scale for Depression in Dementia. A validity study. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 60(5), 360–364. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039480600937066
  • Kraybill, M. L., & Suchy, Y. (2011). Executive functioning, motor programming, and functional independence: Accounting for variance, people, and time. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 25(2), 210–223. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2010.542489
  • Kraybill, M. L., Thorgusen, S. R., & Suchy, Y. (2013). The Push-Turn-Taptap task outperforms measures of executive functioning in predicting declines in functionality: Evidence-based approach to test validation. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 27(2), 238–255. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2012.735702
  • Lezak, M. D., Howieson, D. B., Bigler, E. D., & Tranel, D. (2004). Neuropsychological assessment (5th ed.). Oxford University Press.
  • Li, S. C., Aggen, S., Nesselroade, J., & Baltes, P. (2001). Short-term fluctuations in elderly people’s sensorimotor functioning predict text and spatial memory performance: The Macarthur Successful Aging Studies. Gerontology, 47(2), 100–116. https://doi.org/10.1159/000052782
  • Li, S. C., Huxhold, O., & Schmiedek, F. (2004). Aging and attenuated processing robustness. Evidence from cognitive and sensorimotor functioning. Gerontology, 50(1), 28–34. https://doi.org/10.1159/000074386
  • Liao, W., Zhang, X., Shu, H., Wang, Z., Liu, D., & Zhang, Z. (2017). The characteristic of cognitive dysfunction in remitted late life depression and amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Psychiatry Research, 251, 168–175. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2017.01.024
  • Lindenberger, U., & van Oertzen, T. (2006). Variability in cognitive aging: From taxonomy to theory. In Ellen Bialystock and Fergus I. M. Craik (Eds.) Lifespan cognition: Mechanisms of change (pp. 297–314). Oxford University Press.
  • Lövdén, M., Li, S.-C., Shing, Y. L., & Lindenberger, U. (2007). Within-person trial-to-trial variability precedes and predicts cognitive decline in old and very old age: Longitudinal data from the Berlin Aging Study. Neuropsychologia, 45(12), 2827–2838. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.005
  • Lövdén, M., Schmiedek, F., Kennedy, K. M., Rodrigue, K. M., Lindenberger, U., & Raz, N. (2013). Does variability in cognitive performance correlate with frontal brain volume? NeuroImage, 64, 209–215. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.09.039
  • MacDonald, S. W. S., Hultsch, D. F., & Dixon, R. A. (2003). Performance variability is related to change in cognition: Evidence from the Victoria Longitudinal Study. Psychology and Aging, 18(3), 510–523. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.510
  • MacDonald, S. W. S., Li, S.-C., & Bäckman, L. (2009). Neural underpinnings of within-person variability in cognitive functioning. Psychology and Aging, 24(4), 792–808. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017798
  • Madden, D. J. (2001). Speed and timing of behavioral processes. In J. E. Birren & K. Warner. Schaie (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of aging (5th ed., pp. 288–312). Academic Press.
  • Manard, M., Bahri, M. A., Salmon, E., & Collette, F. (2016). Relationship between grey matter integrity and executive abilities in aging. Brain Research, 1642, 562–580.https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2016.04.04
  • Manly, T., Davison, B., Heutink, J., Galloway, M., & Robertson, I. B. (2000). Not enough time or not enough attention? Speed, error and self-maintained control in the Sustained Attention to Response Test (SART). Clinical Neuropsychological Assessment, 3, 167–177.
  • Martin, M., & Hofer, S. M. (2004). Intraindividual variability, change, and aging: Conceptual and analytical issues. Gerontology, 50(1), 7–11. https://doi.org/10.1159/000074382
  • Mattis, S. (2001). Dementia Rating Scale–2TM. Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • Mella, N., Fagot, D., Lecerf, T., & de Ribaupierre, A. (2015). Working memory and intraindividual variability in processing speed: A lifespan developmental and individual-differences study. Memory & Cognition, 43(3), 340–356. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-014-0491-1
  • Mendonça, M. D., Alves, L., & Bugalho, P. (2016). From Subjective Cognitive Complaints to Dementia: Who is at Risk?: A Systematic Review. American Journal of Alzheimer’s Disease and Other Dementias, 31(2), 105–114. https://doi.org/10.1177/1533317515592331
  • Molton, I. R., & Terrill, A. L. (2014). Overview of persistent pain in older adults. The American Psychologist, 69(2), 197–207. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035794
  • Morcom, A. M., & Johnson, W. (2015). Neural reorganization and compensation in aging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 27(7), 1275–1285. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn_a_00783
  • Morgan, E. E., Woods, S. P., & Grant, I. (2012). Intra-individual neurocognitive variability confers risk of dependence in activities of daily living among HIV-seropositive individuals without HIV-associated neurocognitive disorders. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology : The Official Journal of the National Academy of Neuropsychologists, 27(3), 293–303. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acs003
  • Munoz, E., Stawski, R. S., Sliwinski, M. J., Smyth, J. M., & MacDonald, S. W. S. (2020). The ups and downs of cognitive function: Neuroticism and negative affect drive performance inconsistency. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 75(2), 263–273. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gby032
  • Murtha, S., Cismaru, R., Waechter, R., & Chertkow, H. (2002). Increased variability accompanies frontal lobe damage in dementia. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 8(3), 360–372. https://doi.org/10.1017/s1355617702813170
  • Nesselroade, J. R. (1991). Interindividual differences in intraindividual change. In L. M. Collins & J. L. Horn (Eds.) Best methods for the analysis of change: Recent advances, unanswered questions, future directions (pp. 92–105). American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10099-006
  • Niermeyer, M. A., Franchow, E. I., & Suchy, Y. (2016). Reported expressive suppression in daily life is associated with slower action planning. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 22(6), 671–681. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617716000473
  • Niermeyer, M. A., & Suchy, Y. (2020a). The vulnerability of executive functioning: The additive effects of recent non-restorative sleep, pain interference, and use of expressive suppression on test performance. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(4), 700–719. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2019.1696892
  • Niermeyer, M. A., & Suchy, Y. (2020b). Walking, talking, and suppressing: Executive functioning mediates the relationship between higher expressive suppression and slower dual-task walking among older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(4), 775–796. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2019.1704436
  • Niermeyer, M. A., Suchy, Y., & Ziemnik, R. (2017). Motor sequencing in older adulthood: Relationships with executive functioning and effects of complex. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 31(3), 598–618. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2016.1257071
  • Niermeyer, M. A., Ziemnik, R. E., Franchow, E. I., Barron, C. A., & Suchy, Y. (2019). Greater naturally occurring expressive suppression is associated with poorer executive functioning and motor-sequence learning among older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 41(2), 118–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1502257
  • Ohayon, M. M., & Vecchierini, M.-F. (2005). Normative sleep data, cognitive function and daily living activities in older adults in the community. Sleep, 28(8), 981–989. https://doi.org/10.1093/sleep/28.8.981
  • Oosterman, J. M., de Goede, M., Wester, A. J., van Zandvoort, M. J., & Kessels, R. P. (2011). Perspective taking in Korsakoff’s syndrome: The role of executive functioning and task complexity. Acta Neuropsychiatrica, 23(6), 302–308. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1601-5215.2011.00552.x
  • Pocheptsova, A., Amir, O., Dhar, R., & Baumeister, R. F. (2009). Deciding without resources: Resource depletion and choice in context. Journal of Marketing Research, 46(3), 344–355. https://doi.org/10.1509/jmkr.46.3.344
  • Pruessner, L., Barnow, S., Holt, D. V., Joormann, J., & Schulze, K. (2020). A cognitive control framework for understanding emotion regulation flexibility. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 20(1), 21–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000658
  • Rabbitt, P., Osman, P., Moore, B., & Stollery, B. (2001). There are stable individual differences in performance variability, both from moment to moment and from day to day. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 54(4), 981–1003. https://doi.org/10.1080/713756013
  • Royall, D. R., Palmer, R., Chiodo, L. K., & Polk, M. J. (2004). Declining executive control in normal aging predicts change in functional status: The Freedom House study. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 52(3), 346–352. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2004.52104.x
  • Scheller, E., Minkova, L., Leitner, M., & Klöppel, S. (2014). Attempted and successful compensation in preclinical and early manifest neurodegeneration - a review of task FMRI studies. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 5, 132. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00132
  • Schmeichel, B. J. (2007). Attention control, memory updating, and emotion regulation temporarily reduce the capacity for executive control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 136(2), 241–255. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.2.241
  • Schmeichel, B. J., & Tang, D. (2015). Individual differences in executive functioning and their relationship to emotional processes and responses. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(2), 93–98. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721414555178
  • Schmeichel, B. J., Vohs, K. D., & Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Intellectual performance and ego depletion: Role of the self in logical reasoning and other information processing. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85(1), 33–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.1.33
  • Schmidt, C., Collette, F., Cajochen, C., & Peigneux, P. (2007). A time to think: Circadian rhythms in human cognition. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 755–789. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290701754158
  • Sliwinski, M. J., Smyth, J. M., Hofer, S. M., & Stawski, R. S. (2006). Intraindividual coupling of daily stress and cognition. Psychology and Aging, 21(3), 545–557. https://doi.org/10.1037/0882-7974.21.3.545
  • Soto, J. A., Perez, C. R., Kim, Y.-H., Lee, E. A., & Minnick, M. R. (2011). Is expressive suppression always associated with poorer psychological functioning? A cross-cultural comparison between European Americans and Hong Kong Chinese. Emotion (Washington, D.C.), 11(6), 1450–1455. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023340
  • Stawski, R. S., MacDonald, S. W. S., Brewster, P. W. H., Munoz, E., Cerino, E. S., & Halliday, D. W. R. (2019). A comprehensive comparison of quantifications of intraindividual variability in response times: A measurement burst approach. The Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 74(3), 397–408. https://doi.org/10.1093/geronb/gbx115
  • Stuss, D. T., & Alexander, M. P. (2000). Executive functions and the frontal lobes: A conceptual view. Psychological Research, 63(3–4), 289–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s004269900007
  • Stuss, D. T., Murphy, K. J., Binns, M. A., & Alexander, M. P. (2003). Staying on the job: The frontal lobes control individual performance variability. Brain : A Journal of Neurology, 126(Pt 11), 2363–2380. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg237
  • Suchy, Y. (2015). Executive functioning: A comprehensive guide for clinical practice. Oxford University Press.
  • Suchy, Y., & Brothers, S. (in press). Reliability and validity of composite scores from the timed subtests of the D-KEFS battery. Psychological Assessment.
  • Suchy, Y., Brothers, S., Mullen, C. M., & Niermeyer, M. A. (2020). Chronic versus recent expressive suppression burdens are differentially associated with cognitive performance among older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 42(8), 834–838. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2020.1817862
  • Suchy, Y., Butner, J., Wiebe, D. J., Campbell, M., Turner, S. L., & Berg, C. A. (2020). Executive cognitive functions and behavioral control differentially predict HbA1c in Type 1 diabetes across emerging adulthood. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 26(4), 353–363. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719001310
  • Suchy, Y., Franchow, E. I., Niermeyer, M. A., Ziemnik, R. E., Williams, P. G., & Pennington, N. N. (2018). Exaggerated reaction to novelty as a predictor of incipient cognitive decline among community-dwelling older adults. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 40(10), 987–999. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2018.1457627
  • Suchy, Y., & Kraybill, M. (2007). The relationship between motor programming and executive abilities: Constructs measured by the Push-Turn-Taptap task from the Behavioral Dyscontrol Scale-Electronic Version. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(6), 648–659. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390600910506
  • Suchy, Y., Kraybill, M. L., & Franchow, E. (2011). Practice effect and beyond: Reaction to novelty as an independent predictor of cognitive decline among older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 17(1), 101–111. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561771000130X
  • Suchy, Y., Niermeyer, M. A., Franchow, E. I., & Ziemnik, R. E. (2019a). Naturally Occurring Expressive Suppression is Associated with Lapses in Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Among Community-Dwelling Older Adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 25(7), 718–728. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617719000328
  • Suchy, Y., Niermeyer, M., Franchow, E., & Ziemnik, R. (2019b). The deleterious impact of expressive suppression on test performance persists at one-year follow-up in community-dwelling older adults. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society : JINS, 25(1), 29–38. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617718000838
  • Suchy, Y., Ziemnik, R. E., Niermeyer, M. A., & Brothers, S. L. (2020). Executive functioning interacts with complexity of daily life in predicting daily medication management among older adults. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 34(4), 797–825. https://doi.org/10.1080/13854046.2019.1694702
  • Thorgusen, S. R., Suchy, Y., Chelune, G. J., & Baucom, B. R. (2016). Neuropsychological practice effects in the context of cognitive decline: Contributions from learning and task novelty. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22(4), 453–466. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1355617715001332
  • Tinajero, R., Williams, P. G., Cribbet, M. R., Rau, H. K., Bride, D. L., & Suchy, Y. (2018). Nonrestorative sleep in healthy, young adults without insomnia: Associations with executive functioning, fatigue, and pre-sleep arousal. Sleep Health, 4(3), 284–291. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sleh.2018.02.006
  • Tun, P. A., & Lachman, M. E. (2008). Age differences in reaction time and attention in a national telephone sample of adults: Education, sex, and task complexity matter. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1421–1429. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012845
  • Vaurio, R. G., Simmonds, D. J., & Mostofsky, S. H. (2009). Increased intra-individual reaction time variability in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder across response inhibition tasks with different cognitive demands. Neuropsychologia, 47(12), 2389–2396. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.022
  • Vohs, K. D., & Heatherton, T. F. (2000). Self-regulatory failure: A resource-depletion approach. Psychological Science, 11(3), 249–254. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00250
  • West, R., Murphy, K. J., Armilio, M. L., Craik, F. I. M., & Stuss, D. T. (2002). Lapses of intention and performance variability reveal age-related increases in fluctuations of executive control. Brain and Cognition, 49(3), 402–419. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2001.1507
  • Whelan, R. (2008). Effective analysis of reaction time data. The Psychological Record, 58(3), 475–483. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395630
  • Williams, B. R., Hultsch, D. F., Strauss, E. H., Hunter, M. A., & Tannock, R. (2005). Inconsistency in reaction time across the life span. Neuropsychology, 19(1), 88–96. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.19.1.88
  • Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y., & Kraybill, M. L. (2013). Preliminary evidence for low openness to experience as a pre-clinical marker of incipient cognitive decline in older adults. Journal of Research in Personality, 47(6), 945–951. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2013.09.006
  • Williams, P. G., Suchy, Y., & Rau, H. K. (2009). Individual differences in executive functioning: Implications for stress regulation. Annals of Behavioral Medicine : A Publication of the Society of Behavioral Medicine, 37(2), 126–140. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-009-9100-0
  • Williams, P. G., Tinajero, R., & Suchy, Y. (2017). Executive functioning and health. In Oxford handbooks online (pp. 1–53). Oxford University Press.
  • Yesavage, J. A., Brink, T. L., Rose, T. L., Lum, O., Huang, V., Adey, M., & Leirer, V. O. (1982). Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 17(1), 37–49. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-3956(82)90033-4

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.