679
Views
26
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Coordinated pluralism as a means to facilitate integrative taxonomies of cognition

ORCID Icon
Pages 129-145 | Received 17 Mar 2017, Accepted 17 Mar 2017, Published online: 05 May 2017

References

  • Abend, G. 2016. “What are Neural Correlates Neural Correlates of?” Biosocieties. doi:10.1057/s41292-016-0019-y.
  • American Psychiatric Association. 2013. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: APA.
  • Ankeny, R., and S. Leonelli. 2015. “Repertoires: How to Transform a Project into a Research Community.” Bioscience 65 (7): 701–708. doi: 10.1093/biosci/biv061
  • Ankeny, R., and S. Leonelli. 2016. “Repertoires: A Post-Kuhnian Perspective on Scientific Change and Collaborative Research.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 60: 18–28. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsa.2016.08.003
  • Barch, D. M., M. G. Berman, R. Engle, J. Jones, J. Jonides, A. Macdonald 3rd, D. Nee, T. Redick, and S. Sponheim. 2009. “CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Working Memory.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 35 (1): 136–152. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn153
  • Barch, D., T. Braver, C. Carter, R. Poldrack, and T. Robbins. 2009. “CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Executive Control.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 35 (1): 115–135. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn154
  • Barch, D., and C. Carter. 2007. “Measurement Issues in the Use of Cognitive Neuroscience Tasks in Drug Development for Impaired Cognition in Schizophrenia: A Report of the Second Consensus Building Conference of the CNTRICS Initiative.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 34 (4): 613–618. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn037
  • Bilder, R., A. Howe, and F. Sabb. 2013. “Multilevel Models from Biology to Psychology: Mission Impossible?” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122 (3): 917–927. doi: 10.1037/a0032263
  • Butler, P., S. Silverstein, and S. Dakin. 2008. “Visual Perception and Its Impairment in Schizophrenia.” Biological Psychiatry 64 (1): 40–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.023
  • Carter, C., and D. Barch. 2007. “Cognitive Neuroscience-Based Approaches to Measuring and Improving Treatment Effects on Cognition in Schizophrenia: The CNTRICS Initiative.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 33 (5): 1131–1137. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm081
  • Carter, C., D. Barch, R. Buchanan, E. Bullmore, J. Krystal, J. Cohen, M. Geyer, et al. 2008. “Identifying Cognitive Mechanisms Targeted for Treatment Development in Schizophrenia: An Overview of the First Meeting of the Cognitive Neuroscience Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia Initiative.” Biological Psychiatry 64 (1): 4–10. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.03.020
  • Carter, C., J. Kerns, and J. Cohen. 2004. “Cognitive Neuroscience: Bridging Thinking and Feeling to the Brain, and its Implications for Psychiatry.” In Neurobiology of Mental Illness. 3rd ed., edited by D. Charney, and E. Nestler, 180–189. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Chang, H. 2007. Inventing Temperature: Measurement and Scientific Progress. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Chang, H. 2012. Is Water H2O? Evidence, Realism and Pluralism. Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Cronbach, Lee, and Paul Meehl. 1955. “Construct Validity in Psychological Tests.” Psychological Bulletin 52: 281–302. doi: 10.1037/h0040957
  • Cuthbert, B., and M. Kozak. 2013. “Constructing Constructs for Psychopathology: The NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC).” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 122 (3): 928–937. doi: 10.1037/a0034028
  • Feest, U. 2005. “Operationism in Psychology: What the Debate is About, What the Debate Should Be About.” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 41 (2): 131–149. doi: 10.1002/jhbs.20079
  • Feest, U. 2010. “Concepts as Tools in the Experimental Generation of Knowledge in Cognitive Neuropsychology.” Spontaneous Generations: A Journal for the History and Philosophy of Science 4 (1): 173–190.
  • Feest, U. 2011. “Remembering (Short-Term) Memory: Oscillations of an Epistemic Thing.” Erkenntnis 75 (3): 391–411. doi: 10.1007/s10670-011-9341-8
  • Feest, U. 2016. “The Experimenter’s Regress Reconsidered: Replication, Tacit Knowledge and the Dynamics of Knowledge Generation.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part A 58: 34–45. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsa.2016.04.003
  • Figdor, C. 2011. “Semantics and Metaphysics in Informatics: Toward an Ontology of Tasks.” Topics in Cognitive Science 3: 222–226. doi: 10.1111/j.1756-8765.2011.01133.x
  • Francken, J. C., and M. Slors. 2014. “From Commonsense to Science, and Back: The use of Cognitive Concepts in Neuroscience.” Consciousness and Cognition 29: 248–258. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.019
  • Gold, J., D. Barch, C. Carter, S. Dakin, S. Luck, A. MacDonald III, J. Ragland, et al. 2012. “Clinical, Functional and Intertask Correlations of Measures Developed by the Cognitive Neuroscience Test Reliability and Clinical Applications for Schizophrenia Consortium.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 38 (1): 144–152. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr142
  • Green, M. 1996. “What are the Functional Consequences of Neurocognitive Deficits in Schizophrenia?” American Journal of Psychiatry 153: 321–330. doi: 10.1176/ajp.153.3.321
  • Green, M., R. Kern, and R. Heaton. 2004. “Longitudinal Studies of Cognition and Functional Outcome in Schizophrenia: Implications for MATRICS.” Schizophrenia Research 72 (1): 41–51. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.009
  • Haslam, N. 2013. “Reliability, Validity, and the Mixed Blessings of Operationalism.” In The Oxford Handbook of Philosophy and Psychiatry, edited by K. W. M. Fulford, M. Davies, R. Gipps, G. Graham, J. Sadler, G. Tanghellini, and T. Thornton. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Hempel, C. 1959/1965. “Fundamentals of Taxonomy.” In Aspects of Scientific Explanation and Other Essays, 137–154. New York: Free Press.
  • Hochstein, E. 2012. “Minds, Models and Mechanisms: A New Perspective on Intentional Psychology.” Journal of Experimental & Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 24 (4): 547–557. doi: 10.1080/0952813X.2012.693688
  • Hochstein, E. 2013. “Intentional Models as Essential Scientific Tools.” International Studies in the Philosophy of Science 27 (2): 199–217. doi: 10.1080/02698595.2013.813251
  • Kellert, S., H. Longino, and K. Waters. 2006. Scientific Pluralism. Minnesota: University of Minnesota Press.
  • Kerns, J., K. Nuechterlein, T. Braver, and D. Barch. 2008. “Executive Functioning Component Mechanisms and Schizophrenia.” Biological Psychiatry 64 (1): 26–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.027
  • Khalidi, M. 2013. Natural Categories and Human Kinds: Classification in the Natural and Social Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Krakauer, J., A. Ghazanfar, A. Gomez-Marin, M. MacIver, and D. Poeppel. 2017. “Neuroscience Needs Behavior: Correcting a Reductionist Bias.” Neuron 93 (3): 480–490. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.12.041
  • Leonelli, S. 2009. “On the locality of data and claims about phenomena.” Philosophy of Science 76(5): 737–749. doi: 10.1086/605804
  • Leonelli, S. 2013. “Integrating Data to Acquire New Knowledge: Three Modes of Integration in Plant Science.” Studies in History and Philosophy of Science Part C: Studies in History and Philosophy of Biological and Biomedical Sciences 44 (4): 503–514. doi: 10.1016/j.shpsc.2013.03.020
  • Lilienfeld, S. 2014. “The Research Domain Criteria (RDoC): An Analysis of Methodological and Conceptual Challenges.” Behaviour Research and Therapy 62: 129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.07.019
  • Lilienfeld, S., and M. Treadway. 2016. “Clashing Diagnostic Approaches: DSM-ICD Versus RDoC.” Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 12: 435–463. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093122
  • Luck, S., and J. Gold. 2008. “The Construct of Attention in Schizophrenia.” Biological Psychiatry 64 (1): 34–39. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.02.014
  • Martin, N. 2016. “Towards a Practice Oriented Integration of the Mind-Brain Sciences”. Paper presented at the 25th biennial meeting of the Philosophy of Science Association, Chicago, IL. November 3–5, 2016.
  • Mitchell, S. 2003. Biological Complexity and Integrative Pluralism. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Morris, S., and B. Cuthbert. 2012. “Research Domain Criteria: Cognitive Systems, Neural Circuits, and Dimensions of Behavior.” Dialogues in Clinical Neuroscience 14: 29–37.
  • National Institute of Mental Health, National Advisory Mental Health Council Workgroup on Tasks and Measures for Research Domain Criteria (RDoC). 2016. “Behavioral Assessment of Methods for RDoC Constructs.” https://www.nimh.nih.gov/about/advisory-boards-and-groups/namhc/reports/rdoc_council_workgroup_report_153440.pdf.
  • Nuechterlein, K., S. Luck, C. Lustig, and M. Sarter. 2009. “CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Control of Attention.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 35 (1): 182–196. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn158
  • Piccinini, G., and C. Craver. 2011. “Integrating Psychology and Neuroscience: Functional Analyses as Mechanism Sketches.” Synthese 183 (3): 283–311. doi: 10.1007/s11229-011-9898-4
  • Poland, J., and S. Tekin. 2017. Extraordinary Science and Psychiatry: Responses to the Crisis in Mental Health Research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Poldrack, R. 2010. “Subtraction and Beyond: The Logic of Experimental Designs for Neuroimaging.” In Foundational Issues in Human Brain Mapping, edited by Stephen Hanson, and Martin Bunzl, 147–159. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Poldrack, R., A. Kittur, D. Kalar, E. Miller, C. Seppa, Y. Gil, D. Parker, F. Sabb, and R. Bilder. 2011. “The Cognitive Atlas: Toward a Knowledge Foundation for Cognitive Neuroscience.” Neuroinformatics 5 (17): 1–11.
  • Poldrack, R., and T. Yarkoni. 2016. “From Brain Maps to Cognitive Ontologies: Informatics and the Search for Mental Structure.” Annual Review of Psychology 67: 587–612. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033729
  • Ragland, J., N. Cohen, R. Cools, M. Frank, D. Hannula, and C. Ranganath. 2012. “CNTRICS Imaging Biomarkers Final Task Selection: Long-Term Memory and Reinforcement Learning.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 38 (1): 62–72. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr168
  • Ragland, J., R. Cools, M. Frank, D. Pizzagalli, A. Preston, C. Ranganath, and A. Wagner. 2009. “CNTRICS Final Task Selection: Long-Term Memory.” Schizophrenia Bulletin 35 (1): 197–212. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn134
  • Roediger III, H., Y. Dudai, and S. Fitzpatrick. 2007. Science of Memory: Concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Sanislow, C., D. Pine, K. Quinn, M. Kozak, M. Garvey, R. Heinssen, P. Wang, and B. Cuthbert. 2010. “Developing Constructs for Psychopathology Research: Research Domain Criteria.” Journal of Abnormal Psychology 119 (4): 631–639. doi: 10.1037/a0020909
  • Shadish, W., T. Cook, and D. Campbell. 2002. Experimental and Quasi experimental Designs for Generalized Causal Inference. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.
  • Sochat, V., I. Eisenberg, A. Enkavi, J. Li, P. Bissett, and R. Poldrack. 2016. “The Experiment Factory: Standardizing Behavioral Experiments.” Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00610.
  • Stinson, C. 2016. “Mechanisms in Psychology: Ripping Nature at Its Seams.” Synthese 193 (5): 1585–1614. doi: 10.1007/s11229-015-0871-5
  • Sullivan, J. 2009. “The Multiplicity of Experimental Protocols: A Challenge to Reductionist and Nonreductionist Models of the Unity of Science.” Synthese 167: 511–539. doi: 10.1007/s11229-008-9389-4
  • Sullivan, J. 2010. “Reconsidering ‘Spatial Memory’ and the Morris Water Maze.” Synthese, 177 (2): 261–283. doi: 10.1007/s11229-010-9849-5
  • Sullivan, J. 2014a. “Is the Next Frontier in Neuroscience a Decade of the Mind?” In Brain Theory, edited by Charles Wolfe, 45–67. New York: Palgrave-MacMillan.
  • Sullivan, J. 2014b. “Stabilizing Mental Disorders: Prospects and Problems.” In Classifying Psychopathology: Mental Kinds and Natural Kinds, edited by Harold Kincaid, and Jacqueline Sullivan, 257–281. Boston, MA: MIT Press.
  • Sullivan, J. 2016a. “Construct Stabilization and the Unity of the Mind-Brain Sciences.” Philosophy of Science 83: 662–673. doi: 10.1086/687853
  • Sullivan, J. 2016b. “Stabilizing Constructs Across Different Research Fields as a Way to Foster the Integrative Approach of the Research Domain Criteria Project.” Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 10: 309. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2016.00309.
  • Sullivan, J. 2016c. “Neuroscientific Kinds Through the Lens of Scientific Practice.” In Natural Kinds and Classification in Scientific Practice, edited by Catherine Kendig, 47–56. New York: Routledge.
  • Tabb, K. 2016. “Philosophy of Psychiatry After Diagnostic Kinds.”
  • Theurer, K., and D. Hartner. 2016. “Mechanisms, Networks, and Values in Psychiatry: New Arguments for Old Insights into the Science of (Mental) Pathology”.
  • Turner, J. A., and A. R. Laird. 2012. “The Cognitive Paradigm Ontology: Design and Application.” Neuroinformatics 10: 57–66. doi: 10.1007/s12021-011-9126-x
  • Uttal, W. 2001. The New Phrenology: The Limits of Localizing Cognitive Processes in the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.