165
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The role of joint control in selection responses

Pages 41-48 | Received 31 Jan 2015, Accepted 06 Dec 2015, Published online: 06 Feb 2016

References

  • Carter, D. E., & Eckerman, D. A. (1975). Symbolic matching by pigeons: Rate of learning complex discriminations predicted from simple discriminations. Science, 187, 662–664. doi:10.1126/science.1114318
  • Cohen, L. R., Brady, J., & Lowry, M. (1981). The role of differential responding in matching-to-sample and delayed matching performance. In M. L. Commons & J. A. Nevin (Eds.), Quantitative analysis of behavior: Vol. 1. Discriminative properties of reinforcement schedules (pp. 345–364). Cambridge, MA: Ballinger.
  • DeGraaf, A., & Schlinger, H. D. (2012). The effects of joint control on the acquisition and durability of a sequencing task. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 28, 60–68.
  • Devany, J. M., Hayes, S. C., & Nelson, R. O. (1986). Equivalence class formation in language-able and language-disabled children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 243–257. doi:10.1901/jeab.1986.46-243
  • Dugdale, N., & Lowe, C. F. (1990). Naming and stimulus equivalence. In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behavior analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 115–137). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Guess, D. (1969). A functional analysis of receptive language and productive speech: Acquisition of the plural morpheme. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2, 55–64. doi:10.1901/jaba.1969.2-55
  • Guess, D., & Baer, D. M. (1973). An analysis of individual differences in generalization between receptive and productive language in retarded children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 6, 311–329. doi:10.1901/jaba.1973.6-311
  • Gutierrez, R. D. (2006). The role of rehearsal in joint control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 183–189.
  • Horne, J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241. doi:10.1901/jeab.1996.65-185
  • Lee, V. L. (1981). Prepositional phrases spoken and heard. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 227–242. doi:10.1901/jeab.1981.35-227
  • Lowenkron, B. (1984). Coding responses and the generalization of matching-to-sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 1–18. doi:10.1901/jeab.1984.42-1
  • Lowenkron, B. (1988). Generalization of delayed identity matching in retarded children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50, 163–172. doi:10.1901/jeab.1988.50-163
  • Lowenkron, B. (1989). Instructional control of generalized relational matching to sample in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 293–309. doi:10.1901/jeab.1989.52-293
  • Lowenkron, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–125.
  • Lowenkron, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.
  • Lowenkron, B. (2006a). An introduction to joint control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 123–120.
  • Lowenkron, B. (2006b). Joint control and the selection of stimuli from their description. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 129–149.
  • Lowenkron, B., & Colvin, V. (1992). Joint control and generalized nonidentity matching: Saying when something is not. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 1–9.
  • Lowenkron, B., & Colvin, V. (1995). Generalized instructional control and the production of broadly applicable relational responding. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 12, 12–26.
  • Michael, J. (1985). Two kinds of verbal behavior plus a possible third. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 3, 2–5.
  • Saunders, K. J. (1989). Naming in conditional discrimination and stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 379–384. doi:10.1901/jeab.1989.51-379
  • Sidener, D. (2006). Joint control for dummies: An elaboration of Lowenkron’s model of joint (stimulus) control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 118–122.
  • Sidman, M. (1990). Equivalence relations: Where do they come from? In D. E. Blackman & H. Lejeune (Eds.), Behavior analysis in theory and practice: Contributions and controversies (pp. 93–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Sidman, M. (2000). Equivalence relations and the reinforcement contingency. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 127–146. doi:10.1901/jeab.2000.74-127
  • Sidman, M., & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5–22. doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.37-5
  • Skinner, B. F. (1957/2001). Verbal behavior. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • Tu, J. C. (2006). The role of joint control in the manded selection responses of both vocal and non-vocal children with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 22, 191–206.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.