561
Views
4
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Narrative Engagement and Vicarious Interaction with Multiple Characters

, , , &

References

  • Adachi, P. J., Ryan, R. M., Frye, J., McClurg, D., & Rigby, C. S. (2018). “I can’t wait for the next episode!” Investigating the motivational pull of television dramas through the lens of self-determination theory. Motivation Science, 4(1), 78–94.
  • Albrecht, J. E., & O’Brien, E. J. (1993). Updating a mental model: Maintaining both local and global coherence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19(5), 1061–1070. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.19.5.1061
  • Baker, L. (1989). Metacognition, comprehension monitoring, and the adult reader. Educational Psychologist, 1(1), 3–38. doi:10.1007/BF01326548
  • Bond, B. J., & Calvert, S. L. (2014). Parasocial breakup among young children in the United States. Journal of Children and Media, 8, 474–490.
  • Brunyé, T. T., Ditman, T., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2011). Better you than I: Perspectives and emotion simulation during narrative comprehension. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 23(5), 659–666. doi:10.1080/20445911.2011.559160
  • Brunyé, T. T., Mahoney, C. R., & Taylor, H. A. (2010). Moving through imagined space: Mentally simulating locomotion during spatial description reading. Acta Psychologica, 134(1), 110–124. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.01.003
  • Busselle, R., & Bilandzic, H. (2008). Fictionality and perceived realism in experiencing stories: A model of narrative comprehension and engagement. Communication Theory, 18(2), 255–280. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2885.2008.00322.x
  • Carter, R. F., Ruggels, W. L., Jackson, K. M., & Hefner, M. B. (1973). Application of signaled stopping technique to communication research. In P. Clarke (Ed.), New models for mass communication research (pp. 15–43). Beverly Hills, CA: SAGE.
  • Caughey, J. L. (1978). Artificial social relations in modern America. American Quarterly, 30(1), 70–89. doi:10.2307/2712280
  • Charts, M. (2013). U.S. consumers seen behind the curve in live social chatter about TV. Retrieved from http://www.marketingcharts.com/wp/television/us-consumers-seen-behind-the-curve-in-live-social-chatter-about-tv-28006/
  • Davis, M. H., Soderlund, T., Cole, J., Gadol, E., Kute, M., Myers, M., & Weihing, J. (2004). Cognitions associated with attempts to empathize: How do we imagine the perspective of another? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(12), 1625–1635.
  • Duval, S., & Wicklund, R. A. (1972). A theory of objective self-awareness. New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Ellis, G. J., Streeter, S. K., & Engelbrecht, J. D. (1983). Television characters as significant others and the process of vicarious role taking. Journal of Family Issues, 4(2), 367–384. doi:10.1177/019251383004002007
  • Fincher-Kiefer, R. (1996). Encoding differences between bridging and predictive inferences. Discourse Processes, 22(3), 225–246. doi:10.1080/01638539609544974
  • Frank, S. L., Koppen, M., Noordman, L. G. M., & Vonk, W. (2003). Modeling knowledge-based inferences in story comprehension. Cognitive Science, 27, 875–910.
  • Garrod, S. C., & Sanford, A. J. (1990). Referential processes in reading: Focusing on roles and individuals. In D. A. Balota, G. B. Flores d’Arcais, & K. Rayner (Eds.), Comprehension processes in reading (pp. 465–485). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Gavelek, J. R., & Raphael, T. E. (1985). Metacognition, instruction, and the role of questioning activities. In D. L. Forrest-Pressley & G. K. MacKinnon (Eds.), Metacognition, cognition and human performance (pp. 105–118). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
  • Gernsbacher, M. A. (1995). The structure-building framework: What it is, what it might also be, and why. In B. K. Britton & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Models of text understanding (pp. 289–311). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Graesser, A. C., Singer, M., & Trabasso, T. (1994). Constructing inferences during narrative text comprehension. Psychological Review, 101(3), 371–395. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.101.3.371
  • Hawkins, R. P., Pingree, S., Fitzpatrick, M. A., Thompson, M., & Bauman, I. (1991). Implications of concurrent measures of viewer behavior. Human Communication Research, 17(3), 485–504.
  • Hoffner, C. (1996). Children’s wishful identification and parasocial interaction with favorite television characters. Journal of Broadcasting and Electronic Media, 40, 389–402.
  • Holland, N. N. (1975). Five readers reading. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
  • Honeycutt, J. M., Choi, C. W., & DeBerry, J. R. (2009). Communication apprehension and imagined interactions. Communication Research Reports, 26(3), 228–236. doi:10.1080/08824090903074423
  • Horton, D., & Strauss, A. (1957). Interaction in audience-participation shows. The American Journal of Sociology, 62(6), 579–587. doi:10.1086/222106
  • Johnson, J. M. Q., & Patnoe-Woodley, P. D. (2016). Exploring the influence of parasocial relationships and experiences on radio listeners’ consumer behaviors. Communication Research Reports, 33(1), 40–46. doi:10.1080/08824096.2015.1117440
  • King, A. (1989). Effects of self-questioning training on college students’ comprehension of lectures. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 14(4), 366–381. doi:10.1016/0361-476X(89)90022-2
  • King, A. (1991). Improving lecture comprehension: Effects of a metacognitive strategy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 5(4), 331–346.
  • Kurby, C. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2008). Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 72–79.
  • Kurby, C. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2012). Starting from scratch and building brick by brick in comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 40(5), 812–826. doi:10.3758/s13421-011-0179-8
  • Linderholm, T. (2002). Predictive inference generation as a function of working memory capacity and causal text constraints. Discourse, Processes, 34(3), 259–280.
  • Magliano, J. P., Dijkstra, K., & Zwaan, R. A. (1996). Generating predictive inferences while viewing a movie. Discourse Processes, 22(3), 199–224. doi:10.1080/01638539609544973
  • Magliano, J. P., Taylor, H. A., & Kim, H. J. J. (2005). When goals collide: Monitoring the goals of multiple characters. Memory & Cognition, 33(8), 1357–1367. doi:10.3758/BF03193368
  • McDonald, D. G., & Dimmick, J. (2003). The conceptualization and measurement of diversity. Communication Research, 30(1), 60–79. doi:10.1177/0093650202239026
  • McDonald, D. G., Lin, S.-F., Anderegg, J. J., Na, K., & Dale, K. R. (2014). Time, tweets, uses, and gratifications: The dynamic nature of television viewing orientations. Paper presented to the annual conference of International Communication Association, Seattle, WA.
  • McDonald, D. G., Sarge, M., Lin, S.-F., Collier, J., & Potocki, B. (2015). A role for the self: Media content as triggers for autobiographical memories. Communication Research, 42(1), 3–29. doi:10.1177/0093650212464771
  • McQuail, D., Blumler, J. G., & Browmn, J. (1972). The television audience: A revised perspective. In D. McQuail (Ed.), Sociology of mass communication (pp. 135–165). Middlesex, UK: Penguin.
  • Niehaus, J., & Young, R. M. (2014). Cognitive models of discourse comprehension for narrative generation. Literary and Linguistic Computing, 29(4), 561–582.
  • Noh, S., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2009). Age differences in tracking characters during narrative comprehension. Memory & Cognition, 37(6), 769–778. doi:10.3758/MC.37.6.769
  • Nussbaum, M. C. (1986). The fragility of goodness: Luck and ethics in Greek tragedy and philosophy. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Oatley, K. (1995). A taxonomy of the emotions of literary response and a theory of identification in fictional narrative. Poetics, 23(1), 53–74. doi:10.1016/0304-422X(94)P4296-S
  • Oatley, K. (1999). Meeting of minds: Dialogue, sympathy, and identification in reading fiction. Poetics, 26(5), 439–454. doi:10.1016/S0304-422X(99)00011-X
  • Oatley, K. (2011). Character, action, incident: Mental models of people and their doings. In Such stuff as dreams: The psychology of fiction (pp. 81–106). West Sussex,UK. doi:10.1002/9781119970910.ch4
  • Otero, J. (2009). Question generation and anomaly detection in texts. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (pp. 47-59). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Provalis Research. (2010). WordStat. Montreal, Canada: Author. Version 6.1) [Computer Software].
  • Riedl, M. O., & Young, R. M. (2010). Narrative planning: Balancing plot and character. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 39, 217–268. doi:10.1613/jair.2989
  • Salgado, S., Nienstedt, H. W., & Schneider, L. (2014). Consensus or discussion? An analysis of plurality and consonance in coverage. In R. G. Picard (Ed.), The euro crisis in the medial journalistic coverage of economic crisis and European institutions (pp. 103–124). London, UK: I. B. Tauris.
  • Scott-Rich, S., & Taylor, H. A. (2000). Not all narrative shifts function equally. Journal of Memory & Cognition, 28, 1257–1266.
  • Smallwood, J., McSpadden, M., & Schooler, J. W. (2008). When attention matters: The curious incident of the wandering mind. Memory & Cognition, 36(6), 1144–1150. doi:10.3758/MC.36.6.1144
  • Tan, E. S. H. (2008). Entertainment is emotion: The functional architecture of the entertainment experience. Media Psychology, 11(1), 28–51. doi:10.1080/15213260701853161
  • Therriault, D. J., & Rinck, M. (2007). Multidimensional situation models. In F. Schmalhofer & C. Perfetti (Eds.), Higher level language processes in the brain: Inference and comprehension processes (pp. 311–327). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Tian, Q., & Hoffner, C. A. (2010). Parasocial interaction with liked, neutral, and disliked characters on a popular TV series. Mass Communication and Society, 13(3), 250–269. doi:10.1080/15205430903296051
  • Todd, A. R., Simpson, A. J., & Tamir, D. I. (2016). Active perspective taking induces flexible use of self-knowledge during social inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(22), 1583–1588. doi:10.1037/xge0000237
  • van Dijk, T., & Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of discourse comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Visch, V., & Tan, E. (2008). Narrative versus style: Effect of genre-typical events versus genre-typical filmic realizations on film viewers’ genre recognition. Poetics, 36, 301–315. doi:10.1016/j.poetic.2008.03.003
  • Vorderer, P., Knobloch, S., & Schramm, H. (2001). Does entertainment suffer from interactivity? The impact of watching an interactive TV movie on viewer’s experience of entertainment. Media Psychology, 3(4), 343–363.
  • Wood, J. V., Saltzberg, J. A., & Goldsamt, L. A. (1990). Does affect induce self-focused attention? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(5), 899–908. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.58.5.899
  • Zillmann, D. (1996). The psychology of suspense in dramatic exposition. In P. Vorderer, W. J. Wulff, & M. Friedrichsen (Eds.), Suspense: Conceptualizations, theoretical analyses, and empirical explorations (pp. 199–231). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Zwaan, R. A. (2004). The immersed experiencer: Toward an embodied theory of language comprehension. In B. H. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 35–62). New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Zwaan, R. A., & Radvansky, G. A. (1998). Situation models in language comprehension and memory. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 162–185. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.123.2.162

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.