1,201
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Studying Executive Function in Culturally Meaningful Ways

& ORCID Icon

References

  • Alcalá, L., & Cervera, M. D. (2022). Yucatec Maya mothers' ethnotheories about learning to help at home. Infant and Child Development, 31(4), e2318.
  • Alcalá, L., Cervera, M. D., & Fernandez, Y. S. (2021). How Yucatec Maya children learn to help at home. Human Development, 65(4)(4), 191–203. doi:10.1159/000518457
  • Alcalá, L., Rogoff, B., & López-Fraire, A. (2018). Sophisticated collaboration is common among Mexican-heritage US children. PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 115(45), 11377–11384.
  • Baggetta, P., & Alexander, P. A. (2016). Conceptualization and operationalization of Executive Function. Mind, Brain, and Education, 10(1), 10–33. doi:10.1111/mbe.12100
  • Bailey, R., & Jones, S. M. (2019). An integrated model of regulation for applied settings. Clinical child and family psychology review, 22(1), 2–23. doi:10.1007/s10567-019-00288-y
  • Barker, J. E., Semenov, A. D., Michaelson, L., Provan, L. S., Snyder, H. R., & Munakata, Y. (2014). Less-structured time in children's daily lives predicts self-directed executive functioning. Frontiers in Psychology, 593.
  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81(6), 1641–1660. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x
  • Cervera, M. D. (2016). Studying Yucatec Maya children through the eyes of their mothers. In V. Pache, C.-E. de Suremain, & E. Guillermet (Eds.), Production institutionnelle de l’enfance. Déclinaisons locales et pratiques d’acteurs (Amérique latine et Europe) (pp. 161–177). Liège, Belgium: Presses Universitaires de Liège.
  • Chao, R. K. (1994). Beyond parental control and authoritarian parenting style: Understanding Chinese parenting through the cultural notion of training. Child Development, 65(4), 1111–1119.
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168.
  • Diamond, A., & Ling, D. S. (2016). Conclusions about interventions, programs, and approaches for improving executive functions that appear justified and those that, despite much hype, do not. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 34–48. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2015.11.005
  • Doebel, S. (2020). Rethinking executive function and its development. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(4), 942–956. doi:10.1177/1745691620904771
  • Dutra, N. B., Chen, L., Anum, A., Burger, O., Davis, H. E., Dzokoto, V. A., & Legare, C. H. (2022). Examining relations between performance on non#verbal executive function and verbal self#regulation tasks in demographically#diverse populations. Developmental science, 25(5), e13228.
  • Gaskins, S. (2000). Children’s daily activities in a Mayan village: A culturally grounded description. Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Comparative Social Science, 34(4), 375–389. doi:10.1177/106939710003400405
  • Gaskins, S. (2006). The cultural organization of Yucatec Mayan children’s social interactions. In X. Chen, D. French, & B. Schneider (Eds.), Peer relationships in cultural context (pp. 283–309). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Gaskins, S. (2013). Pretend play as culturally constructed activity. In M. Taylor (Ed.), Oxford handbook on the development of the imagination (pp. 224–247). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gaskins, S. (2020). Integrating Cultural Values Through Everyday Experiences: Yucatec Maya Children’s Moral Development. In L. A. Jensen (Ed.), Handbook of moral development: An interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 186–202). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Gaskins, S. (in press). Maximizing cultural validity in the study of child development. To appear. In D. M. Teti, B. H. Cleveland, & K. Rulison (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in developmental science (2nd ed.). London, UK: Wiley.
  • Gaskins, S., & Miller, P. J. (2009). The cultural roles of emotions in pretend play. In C. D. Clark (Ed.), Transactions at play (pp. 5–21). Lanham, MD: University Press of America (Play and Cultural Studies Series).
  • Gaskins, S., & Paradise, R. (2010). Learning through observation. In D. F. Lancy, J. Bock, & S. Gaskins (Eds.), The Anthropology of Learning in Childhood (pp. 85–117). Lanham, MD: Alta Mira Press.
  • Gaskins, A. L., & Richland, L. E. (2021). A cultural lens on Yucatec Maya families’ COVID-19 experiences. Child Development, 2021, 1–15. doi:10.1111/cdev.13657
  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 61–83. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
  • Jacob, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: A review. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 512–552. doi:10.3102/0034654314561338
  • Jaramillo, J. M., Rendón, M. I., Muñoz, L., Weis, M., & Trommsdorff, G. (2017). Children’s Self-Regulation in Cultural Contexts: The Role of Parental Socialization Theories, Goals, and Practices. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 923. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00923
  • Jiménez-Balam, D. (2021). Concepções e metas de socialização de emoções entre mães e pais mayas do México e análise comparativas com participantes de agrovilas do Brasil [Unpublished Doctoral Thesis of Universidade Federal do Pará]. Universidade Federal de Pará, Pará, Brazil.
  • Jiménez-Balam, D., Alcalá, L., & Salgado, D. (2019). Maya children’s acquisition of medicinal plant knowledge: Initiative and agency in their learning process. Learning, Culture and Social Interaction, 22, 100333. doi:10.1016/j.lcsi.2019.100333
  • Lamm, B., Keller, H., Teiser, J., Gudi, H., Yovsi, R. D., Freitag, C., … Lohaus, A. (2018). Waiting for the second treat: Developing culture‐specific modes of self‐regulation. Child Development, 89(3), e261–e277. doi:10.1111/cdev.12847
  • Lareau, A. (2011). Unequal Childhoods. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
  • Legare, C. H., Dale, M. T., Kim, S. Y., & Deák, G. O. (2018). Cultural variation in cognitive flexibility reveals diversity in the development of executive functions. Scientific reports, 8(1), 1–14. doi:10.1038/s41598-018-34756-2
  • López, A., Ruvalcaba, O., & Rogoff, B. (2014). Attentive helping as a cultural practice of Mexican-heritage families. In Y. M. Caldera & E. Lindsey (Eds.), Mexican American children and families (pp. 96–111). New York, NY: Routledge.
  • Mejía-Arauz, R., Correa-Chávez, M., Ohrt, U. K., & Aceves-Azuara, I. (2015). Collaborative work or individual chores: The role of family social organization in children’s learning to collaborate and develop initiative. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 49, 25–51.
  • Miller‐Cotto, D., Smith, L. V., Wang, A. H., & Ribner, A. D. (2022). Changing the conversation: A culturally responsive perspective on executive functions, minoritized children and their families. Infant and Child Development, 31(1), e2286. doi:10.1002/icd.2286
  • Mischel, W. (2015). The marshmallow test: Why self-control is the engine of success. New York, NY: Little, Brown.
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Müller, U., & Kerns, K. (2015). The development of executive function. In L. S. Liben, U. Müller, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes (pp. 571–623). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.
  • Nesbitt, K. T., Baker-Ward, L., & Willoughby, M. T. (2013). Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 774–783. doi:10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.005
  • Noble, K. G., McCandliss, B. D., & Farah, M. J. (2007). Socioeconomic gradients predict individual differences in neurocognitive abilities. Developmental Science, 10(4), 464–480. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00600.x
  • Obradović, J., & Willoughby, M. T. (2019). Studying executive function skills in young children in low‐and middle‐income countries: Progress and directions. Child Development Perspectives, 13(4), 4. doi:10.1111/cdep.12349
  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive psychology, 66(2), 232–258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002
  • Paradise, R., & de Haan, M. (2009). Responsibility and Reciprocity: Social Organization of Mazahua Learning Practices. Anthropology & education quarterly, 40(2), 187–204. doi:10.1111/j.1548-1492.2009.01035.x
  • Rogoff, B. (2014). Learning by observing and pitching in to family and community endeavors: An orientation. Human Development, 57(2–3), 69–81. doi:10.1159/000356757
  • Rogoff, B., Dahl, A., & Callanan, M. (2018). The importance of understanding children’s lived experience. In Developmental Review 50 (pp. 5–15).
  • Roos, L. E., Beauchamp, K. G., Flannery, J., & Fisher, P. A. (2017). Cultural contributions to childhood executive function. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 8, 61.
  • Sarma, U. A., & Thomas, T. M. (2020). Breaking the limits of executive functions: Towards a sociocultural perspective. Culture and Psychology, 26(3), 358–368. doi:10.1177/1354067X19898673
  • Schirmbeck, K., Rao, N., & Maehler, C. (2020). Similarities and differences across countries in the development of executive functions in children: A systematic review. Infant and Child Development, 29(1), e2164. doi:10.1002/icd.2164
  • Shweder, R. A. (1990). Cultural psychology—what is it? In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural Psychology: Essays on Comparative Human Development (pp. 1–43). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Spiegel, J. A., Goodrich, J. M., Morris, B. M., Osborne, C. M., & Lonigan, C. J. (2021). Relations between executive functions and academic outcomes in elementary school children: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 147(4), 329. doi:10.1037/bul0000322
  • Stucke, N. J., Stoet, G., & Doebel, S. (2022). What are the kids doing? Exploring young children’s activities at home and relations with externally cued executive function and child temperament. Developmental Science, 25(5), e13226. doi:10.1111/desc.13226
  • Tran, C., Arredondo, M., & Yoshido, H. (2019). Early executive function: The influence of culture and bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 714–732. doi:10.1017/S1366728918000160
  • Weisner, T. S. (2018). Culture, context, and the integration of qualitative and quantitative methods in the study of human development. In M. Gelfand, C. Y. Chiu, & Y. Y. Hong (Eds.), Advances in Culture and Psychology (Vol. 7, pp. 153–216). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Whiting, B. (1978). The Dependency Hang-Up and Experiments in Alternative Life Styles. In M. J. Yinger & S. J. Cutler (Eds.), Major Social Issues: A Multidisciplinary View (pp. 217–226). New York, NY: The Free Press.
  • Zelazo, P. D., & Carlson, S. M. (2012). Hot and cool executive function in childhood and adolescence: Development and plasticity. Child Development Perspectives, 6, 354–360.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.