1,724
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Conceptual Advances

Eliminating Genetic Reductionism from Developmental Science

REFERENCES

  • Anastasi, A. (1958). Heredity, environment, and the question “how?” Psychological Review, 65, 197–208. doi:10.1037/h0044895
  • Baltes, P. B. (1997). On the incomplete architecture of human ontogeny: Selection, optimization, and compensation as foundation of developmental theory. American Psychologist, 52, 366–380. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.4.366
  • Baltes, P. B., Lindenberger, U., & Staudinger, U. M. (2006). Life span theory in developmental psychology. In R. M. Lerner (Ed.), Theoretical models of human development. Volume 1 of handbook of child psychology (6th ed., pp. 569–664). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Bateson, P. (2015). Ethology and human development. In W. F. Overton & P. C. Molenaar (Eds.), Theory and method. Vol. 1. Handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., pp. 208–243). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Belsky, J. (2012). The development of human reproductive strategies: Progress and prospects. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 310–316. doi:10.1177/0963721412453588
  • Belsky, J. (2014, November 30). The downside of resilience. Sunday Review. New York Times, p. SR4.
  • Belsky, J., Steinberg, L., & Draper, P. (1991). Childhood experience, interpersonal development, and reproductive strategy: An evolutionary theory of socialization. Child Development, 62, 647–670.
  • Brandtstädter, J. (1998). Action perspectives on human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 807–863). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development: Experiments by nature and design. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Bronfenbrenner, U. (Ed.). (2005). Making human beings human. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
  • Buck v. Bell, 274 U.S. 200 (1927). Retrieved from http://caselaw.lp.findlaw.com/scripts/getcase.pl?court=US&vol=274&invol=200
  • Cole, S. W. (2014). Human social genomics. PLOS Genetics, 10(8), 1–7.
  • Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. New York, NY: Oxford University.
  • Doerr, A. (2009). Three generations of imbeciles are enough. Genomics Law Report. Retrieved from http://www.genomicslawreport.com/index.php/2009/06/25/three-generations-of-imbeciles-are-enough/
  • Elder, G. H., Jr. (1998). The life course and human development. In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 939–991). New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Elder, G. H., Shanahan, M. J., & Jennings, J. A. 2015. Human development in time and place. In M. H. Bornstein & T. Leventhal (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science, volume 4: Ecological settings and processes in developmental systems (7th ed., pp. 6–54). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Ellis, B. J., Schlomer, G. L., Tilley, E. H., & Butler, E. A. (2012). Impact of fathers on risky sexual behavior in daughters: A genetically and environmentally controlled sibling study. Development and Psychopathology, 24, 317–332. doi:10.1017/S095457941100085X
  • Feldman, M. (2014). Echoes of the past: Hereditarianism and a troublesome inheritance. PLoS Genetics, 10(12), e1004817. doi:10.1371/journal.pgen.1004817
  • Fisher, C. B., Busch-Rossnagel, N. A., Jopp, D. S., & Brown, J. L. (2012). Applied developmental science, social justice and socio-political well-being. Applied Developmental Science, 16, 54–64. doi:10.1080/10888691.2012.642786
  • Ford, D. H., & Lerner, R. M. (1992). Developmental systems theory: An integrative approach. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.
  • Freedman, D. G. (1979). Human sociobiology: A holistic approach. New York, NY: Free Press.
  • Gould, S. J., & Lewontin, R. C. (1979). The spandrels of San Marco and the Panglossian paradigm: A critique of the adaptionist programme. In J. Maynard Smith & R. Holliday (Eds.), The evolution of adaptation by natural selection (pp. 581–598). London, England: Royal Society of London.
  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, C. (1994). The bell curve: Intelligence and class structure in American life. New York, NY: Free Press.
  • Hirsch, J. (1981). To “unfrock the charlatans”. Sage Race Relations Abstracts, 6, 1–65.
  • Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2005). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Jensen, A. R. (1969). How much can we boost IQ and scholastic achievement? Harvard Educational Review, 39, 1–123. doi:10.17763/haer.39.1.l3u15956627424k7
  • Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in mental testing. New York, NY: Free Press.
  • Johnson, C. G. (2013). Female inmates sterilized in California prisons without approval. The Center for Investigative Reporting. Retrieved from http://cironline.org/reports/female-inmates-sterilized-california-prisons-without-approval-4917
  • Keller, E. F. (2010). The mirage of a space between nature and nurture. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
  • Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Quarterly Review of Biology, 28, 337–363.
  • Lehrman, D. S. (1970). Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature–nurture problem. In L. R. Aronson, E. Tobach, D. S. Lehrman, & J. S. Rosenblatt (Eds.), Development and evolution of behavior: Essays in memory of T. C. Schneirla (pp. 17–52). San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman.
  • Lerner, R. M. (1976). Concepts and theories of human development. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Lerner, R. M. (1978). Nature, nurture, and dynamic interactionism. Human Development, 21, 1–20. doi:10.1159/000271572
  • Lerner, R. M. (1984). On the nature of human plasticity. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Lerner, R. M. (2006). Developmental science, developmental systems, and contemporary theories of human development. In R. M. Lerner, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (6th ed., pp. 1–17). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
  • Lerner, R. M. (2012). Essay review: Developmental science: Past, present, and future. International Journal of Developmental Science, 6(1–2), 29–36.
  • Lorenz, K. (1939). Über Ausfallerserscheinungen im Instinctverhalten von Haustieren und ihre socialpsychologische Bedeutung [About deficits in the instinctive behavior of pets and their socio-psychological significance]. In O. Klemm (Ed.), Charackter und Erziehung: 16. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Bayreuth (pp. 139–147). Leipzig, Germany: J. A. Barth.
  • Lorenz, K. (1940a). Durch Domestikation verursachte Störungen arteigenen Verhaltens [Diseases caused by domestication disturbances of species-specific behavior]. Zeitschrift Für Angewandte Psychologie Und Charakterkunde, 59, 2–81.
  • Lorenz, K. (1940b). Systematik und Entwicklungsgedanke im Unterricht [Systematics and development ideas in the classroom]. Der Biologe, 9, 24–36.
  • Lorenz, K. (1966). On aggression. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace & World.
  • Lorenz, K. (1974, September). Letter: Lorenz clarifies ideas. Human Behavior, 6.
  • McCrae, R. R., Costa, P. T., Hrebickova, M., Ostendord, F., Angleitner, A., & Avia, M. D. (2000). Nature over nurture: Temperament, personality, and life span. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(1), 173–186.
  • Meaney, M. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene × environment interactions. Child Development, 81(1), 41–79. doi:10.1111/cdev.2010.81.issue-1
  • Meaney, M. (2014, October 10). Epigenetics offer hope for disadvantaged children [Children and Family web log post]. Retrieved from http://childandfamilyblog.com/epigenetics-offer-hope-disadvantaged-children/
  • Misteli, T. (2013). The cell biology of genomes: Bringing the double helix to life. Cell, 152, 1209–1212. doi:10.1016/j.cell.2013.02.048
  • Molenaar, P. C. M. (2014). Dynamic models of biological pattern formation have surprising implications for understanding the epigenetics of development. Research in Human Development, 11(1), 50–62.
  • Molenaar, P. C. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2012). Merging the idiographic filter with dynamic factor analysis to model process. Applied Developmental Science, 16(4), 210–219.
  • Molenaar, P. C., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2014). New trends in the inductive use of relation developmental systems theory: Ergodicity, nonstationarity, and heterogeneity. In P. C. Molenaar, R. M. Lerner, & K. M. Newell (Eds.), Handbook of developmental systems and methodology (pp. 442–462). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Molenaar, P. C. M., & Nesselroade, J. R. (2015). Systems methods for developmental research. In W. F. Overton, & P. C. Molenaar (Eds.), Theory and method. Volume 1 of the handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., pp. 652–682). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Molenaar, P. C. M., Newell, K., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.). (2014). Handbook of developmental systems theory and methodology. New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Nesselroade, J. R. (1988). Some implications of the trait-state distinction for the study of development over the life-span: The case of personality. In P. B. Baltes, D. L. Featherman, & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Life-span development and behavior (Vol. 8, pp. 163–189). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Nesselroade, J. R., & Molenaar, P. C. M. (2010). Emphasizing intraindividual variability in the study of development over the life span. In W. F. Overton (Ed.), The handbook of life-span development. Vol. 1: Cognition, biology, methods (pp. 30–54). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Overton, W. F. (1973). On the assumptive base of the nature–nurture controversy: Additive versus interactive conceptions. Human Development, 16, 74–89. doi:10.1159/000271268
  • Overton, W. F. 2015. Process and relational developmental systems. In W. F. Overton & P. C. Molenaar (Eds.), Theory and method. Volume 1 of the handbook of child psychology and developmental science (7th ed., pp. 9–62). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Pigliucci, M., & Mueller, G. B. (2010). Elements of an extended evolutionary synthesis. In M. Pigliucci & G. B. Mueller (Eds.), Evolution – The extended synthesis (pp. 3–17). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Riegel, K. F. (1975). Toward a dialectical theory of development. Human Development, 18, 50–64. doi:10.1159/000271475
  • Riegel, K. F. (1976). The dialectics of human development. American Psychologist, 31, 689–700. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.31.10.689
  • Rushton, J. P. (1992). Cranial capacity related to sex, rank, and race in a stratified random sample of 6,325 U.S. military personnel. Intelligence, 16, 401–413. doi:10.1016/0160-2896(92)90017-L
  • Rushton, J. P. (2000). Race, evolution, and behavior (2nd ed.). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.
  • Schneirla, R. C. (1956). Interrelationships of the innate and the acquired in instinctive behavior. In P. P. Grassé (Ed.), L’instinct dans le comportement des animaux et de l’homme (pp. 387–452). Paris, France: Mason et Cie.
  • Schneirla, T. C. (1957). The concept of development in comparative psychology. In D. B. Harris (Ed.), The concept of development: An issue in the study of human behavior (pp. 78–108). Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.
  • Schneirla, T. C. (1966). Instinct and aggression: Reviews of Konrad Lorenz. In Evolution and modification of behavior Chicago, IL: The University of Chicago Press, 1965. On aggression (New York: Harcourt, Brace & World, 1966). Natural History, 75, 16.
  • Slavich, G. M., & Cole, S. W. (2013). The emerging field of human social genomics. Clinical Psychological Science, 1, 331–348.
  • Von Bertalanffy, L. (1933). Modern theories of development. London, England: Oxford University Press.
  • Wade, N. (2014). A troublesome inheritance: Genes, race and human history. New York, NY: Penguin Books.
  • West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Whitehead, A. N. (1978). Process and reality: Corrected edition. New York, NY: Free Press. ( Original work published 1929)
  • Witherington, D. C. (2014). Self-organization and explanatory pluralism: Avoiding the snares of reductionism in developmental science. Research in Human Development, 11(1), 22–36. doi:10.1080/15427609.2014.874763

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.