277
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The Genetic and Environmental Etiology of the Association between Vocabulary and Syntax in First Grade

, , , , , & show all
Pages 149-166 | Received 06 Sep 2016, Accepted 11 Dec 2017, Published online: 09 Mar 2018

References

  • Adams, A.-M., & Gathercole, S. E. (2000). Limitations in working memory: Implications for language development. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 35, 95–116.
  • Baddeley, A., Gathercole, S., & Papagno, C. (1998). The phonological loop as a language learning device. Psychological Review, 105, 158–173.
  • Bearden, C. E., Karlsgodt, K. H., Bachman, P., Van Erp, T. G. M., Winkler, A. M., & Glahn, D. C. (2012). Genetic architecture of declarative memory: Implications for complex illnesses. The Neuroscientist, 18, 516–532.
  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2003). The Test for Reception of Grammar (2nd ed.). London, United Kingdom: Psychological Corporation.
  • Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Genes, cognition, and communication: Insights from neurodevelopmental disorders. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1156, 1–18.
  • Boivin, M., Brendgen, M., Dionne, G., Dubois, L., Pérusse, D., Robaey, P., … Vitaro, F. (2013). The Quebec Newborn Twin Study into adolescence: 15 years later. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 64–69.
  • Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S.-Y., Pascual, L., … Vyt, A. (2004). Crosslinguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development, 75, 1115–1139.
  • Bracken, B. A., & Murray, A. M. (1984). Stability and predictive validity of the PPVT-R over an eleven month interval. Educational & Psychological Research, 4, 41–44.
  • Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Burchinal, M., Howes, C., Pianta, R., Bryant, D., Early, D., Clifford, R., & Barbarin, O. (2008). Predicting child outcomes at the end of kindergarten from the quality of pre-kindergarten teacher–child interactions and instruction. Applied Developmental Science, 12, 140–153.
  • Dale, P. S., Dionne, G., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2000). Lexical and grammatical development: A behavioural genetic perspective. Journal of Child Language, 27, 619–642.
  • Dale, P. S., Harlaar, N., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., & Plomin, R. (2010). The etiology of diverse receptive language skills at 12 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 53, 982–992.
  • Desmottes, L., Meulemans, T., & Maillart, C. (2016). Implicit spoken words and motor sequences learning are impaired in children with specific language impairment. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 22, 520–529.
  • DeThorne, L. S., Harlaar, N., Petrill, S. A., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2012). Longitudinal stability in genetic effects on children’s conversational language productivity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 739–753.
  • DeThorne, L. S., Petrill, S. A., Hart, S. A., Channell, R. W., Campbell, R. J., Deater-Deckard, K., … Vandenbergh, D. J. (2008). Genetic effects on children’s conversational language use. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 51, 423–435.
  • Dionne, G., Dale, P. S., Boivin, M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Genetic evidence for bidirectional effects of early lexical and grammatical development. Child Development, 74, 394–412.
  • Dunn, L. M., Thériault-Whalen, C. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1993). Échelle de vocabulaire en image Peabody: Adaptation française du Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised [Échelle de vocabulaire en image Peabody: French adaptation of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-Revised]. Toronto, Canada: Psycan.
  • Ellis, N. C., & Sinclair, S. G. (1996). Working memory in the acquisition of vocabulary and syntax: Putting language in good order. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 49, 234–250.
  • Erickson, L. C., & Thiessen, E. D. (2015). Statistical learning of language: Theory, validity, and predictions of a statistical learning account of language acquisition. Developmental Review, 37, 66–108.
  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Bates, E., Reznick, J. S., Thal, D. J., & Pethick, S. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59, 1–173.
  • Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efficiency and vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98–116.
  • Frank, I., Poulin-Dubois, D., & Trudeau, N. (1997). Inventaire MacArthur-Bates du développement de la communication: Mots et énoncés. Retrieved from http://coder-eoa.eoa.umontreal.ca/Web/MacArthur_Bates/telechargements/IMBDC_mots_et_enonces.pdf.
  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., Corley, R. P., & Hewitt, J. K. (2008). Individual differences in executive functions are almost entirely genetic in origin. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 201–225.
  • Gleitman, L. (1990). The structural sources of verb meanings. Language Acquisition, 1, 3–55.
  • Harlaar, N., Meaburn, E. L., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Davis, O. S. P., Docherty, S., & Plomin, R., (2014). Genome-wide association study of receptive language ability of 12-year-olds. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 96–105.
  • Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Dale, P. S., & Plomin, R. (2012). The etiology of variation in language skills changes with development: A longitudinal twin study of language from 2 to 12 years. Developmental Science, 15, 233–249.
  • Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Kovas, Y., Harlaar, N., Plomin, R., Bishop, D. V. M., & Dale, P. S. (2006). Common aetiology for diverse language skills in 4 ½-year-old twins. Journal of Child Language, 33, 339–368.
  • Huttenlocher, J., Levine, S., & Vevea, J. (1998). Environmental input and cognitive growth: A study using time-period comparisons. Child Development, 69, 1012–1029.
  • Huttenlocher, J., Vasilyeva, M., Cymerman, E., & Levine, S. (2002). Language input and child syntax. Cognitive Psychology, 45, 337–374.
  • Joseph, R. M., McGrath, L. M., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2005). Executive dysfunction and its relation to language ability in verbal school-age children with autism. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 361–378.
  • Kovas, Y., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Oliver, B., Dale, P. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2005). Genetic influences in different aspects of language development: The etiology of language skills in 4.5-year-old twins. Child Development, 76, 632–651.
  • Lee, L. L. (1974). Developmental sentence analysis: A grammatical assessment procedure for speech and language clinicians. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.
  • Lever, R., & Sénéchal, M. (2011). Discussing stories: On how a dialogic reading intervention improves kindergartners’ oral narrative construction. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 1–24.
  • Lobo, M. K., Karsten, S. L., Gray, M., Geschwind, D. H., & Yang, X. W. (2006). FACS-array profiling of striatal projection neuron subtypes in juvenile and adult mouse brains. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 443–452.
  • Lum, J. A. G., Conti-Ramsden, G., Page, D., & Ullman, M. T. (2012). Working, declarative and procedural memory in specific language impairment. Cortex, 48, 1138–1154.
  • MacWhinney, B. (1987). The competition model. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), Mechanisms of language acquisition (pp. 249–308). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Malvern, D., Richards, B., Chipere, N., & Durán, P. (2004). Lexical diversity and language development: Quantification and assessment. Basingstoke, United Kingdom: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Mashburn, A. J., Justice, L. M., Downer, J. T., & Pianta, R. C. (2009). Peer effects on children’s language achievement during pre-kindergarten. Child Development, 80, 686–702.
  • McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325–343.
  • Mimeau, C., Plourde, V., Ouellet, -A.-A., & Dionne, G. (2015). Comparison of measures of morphosyntactic complexity in French-speaking school-aged children. First Language, 35, 163–181.
  • Morrison, F. J., Smith, L., & Dow-Ehrensberger, M. (1995). Education and cognitive development: A natural experiment. Developmental Psychology, 31, 789–799.
  • Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A. J., … Monaco, A. P. (2011). Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects. Behavior Genetics, 41, 90–104.
  • Olson, R. K., Keenan, J. M., Byrne, B., Samuelsson, S., Coventry, W. L., Corley, R., … Hulslander, J. (2011). Genetic and environmental influences on vocabulary and reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 26–46.
  • Peña, M., Bonatti, L. L., Nespor, M., & Mehler, J. (2002). Signal-driven computations in speech processing. Science, 298, 604–607.
  • Perruchet, P., & Pacton, S. (2006). Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 233–238.
  • Pinker, S. (1984). Language learnability and language development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Plomin, R., & Kovas, Y. (2005). Generalist genes and learning disabilities. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 592–617.
  • Preacher, K. J., & Coffman, D. L. (2006). Computing power and minimum sample size for RMSEA. Retrieved from http://quantpsy.org/rmsea/rmsea.htm.
  • Pungello, E. P., Iruka, I. U., Dotterer, A. M., Mills-Koonce, R., & Reznick, J. S. (2009). The effects of socioeconomic status, race, and parenting on language development in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 45, 544–557.
  • Richardson, K., & Norgate, S. (2005). The equal environments assumption of classical twin studies may not hold. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 75, 339–350.
  • Rondal, J. A. (1997). L’évaluation du langage [The evaluation of language]. Sprimont, Belgium: Mardaga.
  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 1926–1928.
  • Saffran, J. R., & Thiessen, E. D. (2007). Domain-general learning capacities. In E. Hoff, & M. Shatz (Eds.), Blackwell Handbook of Language Development (pp. 68–86). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
  • Saffran, J. R., & Wilson, D. P. (2003). From syllables to syntax: Multilevel statistical learning by 12-month-old infants. Infancy, 4, 273–284.
  • Samuelsson, S., Byrne, B., Quain, P., Wadsworth, S., Corley, R., DeFries, J. C., … Olson, R. (2005). Environmental and genetic influences on prereading skills in Australia, Scandinavia, and the United States. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 705–722.
  • Scott, C. M., & Stokes, S. L. (1995). Measures of syntax in school-age children and adolescents. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 26, 309–319.
  • Sénéchal, M., Pagan, S., Lever, R., & Ouellette, G. P. (2008). Relations among the frequency of shared reading and 4-year-old children’s vocabulary, morphological and syntax comprehension, and narrative skills. Early Education and Development, 19, 27–44.
  • Siegler, R. S. (1996). Emerging minds: The process of change in children’s thinking. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Smith, A. (1997). Development and course of receptive and expressive vocabulary from infancy to old age: Administrations of the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test, third edition, and the Expressive Vocabulary Test to the same standardization population of 2725 subjects. International Journal of Neuroscience, 92, 73–78.
  • Snow, C. E. (2010). Academic language and the challenge of reading for learning about science. Science, 328, 450–452.
  • Thordardottir, E. (2005). Early lexical and syntactic development in Quebec French and English: Implications for cross-linguistic and bilingual assessment. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 40, 243–278.
  • Ullman, M. T. (2004). Contributions of memory circuits to language: The declarative/procedural model. Cognition, 92, 231–270.
  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.
  • Weizman, Z. O., & Snow, C. E. (2001). Lexical input as related to children’s vocabulary acquisition: Effects of sophisticated exposure and support for meaning. Developmental Psychology, 37, 265–279.
  • Wright, M., De Geus, E., Ando, J., Luciano, M., Posthuma, D., Ono, Y., … Boomsma, D. (2001). Genetics of cognition: Outline of a collaborative twin study. Twin Research, 4, 48–56.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.