382
Views
2
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Research Article

Cross-generational Phonetic Alignment between Mothers and Their Children

, &

References

  • Ashton-James, C., Van Baaren, R. B., Chartrand, T. L., Decety, J., & Karremans, J. (2007). Mimicry and me: The impact of mimicry on self-construal. Social Cognition, 25(4), 518–535. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2007.25.4.518
  • Babel, M. (2010). Dialect divergence and convergence in New Zealand English. Language in Society, 39(4), 437–456. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404510000400
  • Babel, M. (2012). Evidence for phonetic and social selectivity in spontaneous phonetic imitation. Journal of Phonetics, 40(1), 177–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.09.001
  • Babel, M., & Bulatov, D. (2012). The role of fundamental frequency in phonetic accommodation. Language and Speech, 55(2), 231–248. https://doi.org/10.1177/0023830911417695
  • Babel, M., McGuire, G., Walters, S., & Nicholls, A. (2014). Novelty and social preference in phonetic accommodation. Laboratory Phonology, 5(1), 123–150. https://doi.org/10.1515/lp-2014-0006
  • Barbu, S., Martin, N., & Chevrot, J.-P. (2014). The maintainence of regional dialects: A matter of gender? Boys, but not girls, use local varieties in relation to their friends’ nativeness and local identity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1251. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01251
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68(3), 255–278. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., & Walker, S. (2015). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. Journal of Statistical Software, 67(1), 1–48. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v067.i01
  • Bock, J. K. (1986). Meaning, sound, and syntax: Lexical priming in sentence production. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12(4), 575–586. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.12.4.575
  • Bornstein, M. H., Cote, L. R., Maital, S., Painter, K., Park, S.-Y., Pascual, L., Pêcheux, M.-G., Ruel, J., Venuti, P., & Vyt, A. (2004). Cross-linguistic analysis of vocabulary in young children: Spanish, Dutch, French, Hebrew, Italian, Korean, and American English. Child Development, 75(4), 1115–1139. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00729.x
  • Bourhis, R. Y., & Giles, H. (1977). The language of intergroup distinctiveness. In H. Giles (Ed.), Language, ethnicity, and intergroup relations (pp. 119–135). Academic Press.
  • Branigan, H. P., Tosi, A., & Gillespie-Smith, K. (2016). Spontaneous lexical alignment in children with an autistic spectrum disorder and their typically developing peers. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(11), 1821–1831. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000272
  • Brennan, S. E., & Clark, H. H. (1996). Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(6), 1482–1493. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.22.6.1482
  • Buttelmann, D., Zmyj, N., Daum, M., & Carpenter, M. (2013). Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(2), 422–428. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x
  • Chambers, J. K. (1992). Dialect acquisition. Language, 68(4), 673–705. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.1992.0060
  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
  • Clopper, C. G., & Dossey, E. (2020). Phonetic convergence to Southern American English: Acoustics and perception. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 147(1), 671–683. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0000555
  • Deser, T. (1989). Dialect transmission and variation: An acoustic analysis of vowels in six urban detroit families. York Papers in Linguistics, 13, 115–128. https://www.york.ac.uk/language/ypl/ypl1/13.html
  • Dufour, S., & Nguyen, N. (2013). How much imitation is there in a shadowing task? Frontiers in Psychology, 4, Article 346. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00346
  • Eaton, C. T., & Ratner, N. B. (2013). Rate and phonological variation in preschool children: Effects of modeling and directed influence. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56(6), 1751–1763. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2013/12-0171)
  • Eckert, P. (2000). Linguistic variation as social practice: The linguistic construction of identity in belten high. Blackwell Publishers.
  • Evans, B. G., & Iverson, P. (2007). Plasticity in vowel perception and production: A study of accent change in young adults. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121(6), 3814–3826. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2722209
  • Fischer, J. L. (1958). Social influences on the choice of a linguistic variant. WORD, 14(1), 47–56. https://doi.org/10.1080/00437956.1958.11659655
  • Foulkes, P., & Docherty, G. (2006). The social life of phonetics and phonology. Journal of Phonetics, 34(4), 409–438. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2005.08.002
  • Foulkes, P., Docherty, G. J., & Watt, D. (2005). Phonological variation in child-directed speech. Language, 81(1), 177–206. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2005.0018
  • Fung, P., Schertz, J., & Johnson, E. K. (2021). The development of gendered speech in children: Insights from adult L1 and L2 perceptions. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America Express Letters, 1(1), 014407. https://doi.org/10.1121/10.0003322
  • Garnier, M., Lamalle, L., & Sato, M. (2013). Neural correlates of phonetic convergence and speech imitation. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(Article), 600. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00600
  • Garrod, S., & Anderson, A. (1987). Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination. Cognition, 27(2), 181–218. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)90018-7
  • Garvey, C., & BenDebba, M. (1974). Effects of age, sex, and partner on children’s dyadic speech. Child Development, 45(4), 1159–1161. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128114
  • Goldinger, S. D. (1998). Echoes of echoes? An Episodic Theory of Lexical Access. Psychological Review, 105(2), 251–279. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.105.2.251
  • Gregory, S. W., Jr., & Webster, S. (1996). A nonverbal signal in voices of interview partners effectively predicts communication accommodation and social status perceptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70(6), 1231–1240. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.70.6.1231
  • Gros-Louis, J., West, M. J., Goldstein, M. H., & King, A. P. (2006). Mothers provide differential feedback to infants’ prelinguistic sounds. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(6), 509–516. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025406071914
  • Harrington, J. (2007). Evidence for a relationship between synchronic variability and diachronic change in the Queen’s annual Christmas broadcasts. In J. Cole & J. Hualde (Eds.), Laboratory phonology 9: Phonetics and phonology (pp. 125–144). Walter de Gruyter.
  • Hines, M. (2015). Gendered development. In M. E. Lamb & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Socioemotional processes (pp. 842–887). John Wiley & Sons Inc. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy320
  • Imafuku, M., Kanakogi, Y., Butler, D., & Myowa, M. (2019). Demystifying infant vocal imitation: The roles of mouth looking and speaker’s gaze. Developmental Science, 22(6), Article e12825. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12825
  • Jones, S. S. (2009). The development of imitation in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364(1528), 2325–2335. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0045
  • Karrass, J., Braungart-Rieker, J. M., Mullins, J., & Lefever, J. B. (2002). Processes in language acquisition: The roles of gender, attention, and maternal encouragement of attention over time. Journal of Child Language, 29(3), 519–543. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000902005196
  • Kent, R. D. (1976). Anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: Evidence from acoustic studies. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 19(3), 421–447. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.1903.421
  • Kerswill, P., & Williams, A. (2000). Creating a new town koine: Children and language change in Milton Keynes. Language in Society, 29(1), 65–115. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404500001020
  • Khattab, G. (2003). Age, input, and language mode factors in the acquisition of VOT by English-Arabic bilingual children. In M. J. Solé, D. Recasens, & J. Romero (Eds.), Proceedings of the 15th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences, Barcelona, Spain (pp. 3213–3216).
  • Kim, M., Horton, W. S., & Bradlow, A. R. (2011). Phonetic convergence in spontaneous conversations as a function of interlocutor language distance. Laboratory Phonology, 2(1), 125–156. https://doi.org/10.1515/labphon.2011.004
  • Kinzler, K. D., Corriveau, K. H., & Harris, P. L. (2011). Children’s selective trust in native-accented speakers. Developmental Science, 14(1), 106–111. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.00965.x
  • Kitamura, C., Thanavishuth, C., Burnham, D., & Luksaneeyanawin, S. (2002). Universality and specificity in infant-directed speech: Pitch modifications as a function of infant age and sex in a tonal and a non-tonal language. Infant Behavior & Development, 24(4), 372–392. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0163-6383(02)00086-3
  • Klinger, H. (1962). Imitated English cleft palate speech in a normal Spanish speaking child. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 27(4), 379–381. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.2704.397
  • Ko, E.-S., Seidl, A., Cristia, A., Reimchen, M., & Soderstrom, M. (2016). Entrainment of prosody in the interaction of mothers with their young children. Journal of Child Language, 43(2), 284–309. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000915000203
  • Kokkinaki, T., & Kugiumutzakis, G. (2000). Basic aspects of vocal imitation in infant-parent interaction during the first 6 months. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 18(3), 173–187. https://doi.org/10.1080/713683042
  • Kuhl, P. K., Conboy, B. T., Coffey-Corina, S., Padden, D., Rivera-Gaxiola, M., & Nelson, T. (2008). Phonetic learning as a pathway to language: New data and native language magnet theory expanded (NLM-e). Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 363(1493), 979–1000. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2154
  • Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). Infant vocalizations in response to speech: Vocal imitation and developmental change. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 100(4), 2425–2438. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.417951
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). lmerTest package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software, 82(13), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v082.i13
  • Labov, W. (2001). Principles of linguistic change:Social factors. Blackwell.
  • Ladegaard, H. J., & Bleses, D. (2003). Gender differences in young children’s speech: The acquisition of sociolinguistic competence. International Journal of Applied Linguistics, 13(2), 222–233. https://doi.org/10.1111/1473-4192.00045
  • Legerstee, M. (1990). Infants use multimodal information to imitate speech sounds. Infant Behavior & Development, 13(3), 343–354. https://doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(90)90039-B
  • Levy, H., & Hanulíková, A. (2019). Variation in children’s vowel production: Effects of language exposure and lexical frequency. Laboratory Phonology: Journal of the Association for Laboratory Phonology, 10(1), 9. https://doi.org/10.5334/labphon.131
  • Lewandowski, E. M., & Nygaard, L. C. (2018). Vocal alignment to native and non-native speakers of English. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 144(2), 620–633. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.5038567
  • Lewandowski, N., & Jilka, M. (2019). Phonetic convergence, language talent, personality and attention. Frontiers in Communication, 4, Article 18. https://doi.org/10.3389/fcomm.2019.00018
  • MacDonald, E. N., Johnson, E. K., Forsythe, J., Plante, P., & Munhall, K. G. (2012). Children’s development of self-regulation in speech production. Current Biology, 22(2), 113–117. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.052
  • Masur, E. F. (1993). Transitions in representational ability: Infants’ verbal, vocal, and action imitation during the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 39(4), 437–456. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23087243
  • Masur, E. F., & Eichorst, D. L. (2002). Infants’ spontaneous imitation of novel versus familiar words: Relations to observational and maternal report measures of their lexicons. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 48(4), 405–426. https://doi.org/10.1353/mpq.2002.0019
  • Masur, E. F., & Olson, J. (2008). Mothers’ and infants’ responses to their partners’ spontaneous action and vocal/verbal imitation. Infant Behavior & Development, 31(4), 704–715. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2008.04.005
  • Masur, E. F., & Rodemaker, J. E. (1999). Mothers’ and infants’ spontaneous vocal, verbal, and action imitation during the second year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45(3), 392–412. https://www.jstor.org/stable/23092579
  • McRoberts, G. W., & Best, C. T. (1997). Accomodation in mean f0 during mother-infant and father-infant vocal interaction: A longitudinal case study. Journal of Child Language, 24(3), 719–736. https://doi.org/10.1017/S030500099700322X
  • Messum, P. (2007). Mirroring, not imitation, for the early learning of L1 pronunciation. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 122(5), 2997. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2942701
  • Messum, P., & Howard, I. S. (2015). Creating the cognitive form of phonological units: The speech sound correspondence problem in infancy could be solved by mirrored vocal interactions rather than by imitation. Journal of Phonetics, 53, 125–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2015.08.005
  • Miller, R. M., Sanchez, K., & Rosenblum, L. D. (2013). Is speech alignment to talkers or tasks? Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 75(8), 1817–1826. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-013-0517-y
  • Milroy, L. (1980). Language and social networks. Basil Blackwell.
  • Munro, M. J., Derwing, T. M., & Flege, J. E. (1999). Canadians in Alabama: A perceptual study of dialect acquisition in adults. Journal of Phonetics, 27(4), 385–403. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1999.0101
  • Munson, B., Crocker, L., Pierrehumbert, J. B., Owen-Anderson, A., & Zucker, K. J. (2015). Gender typicality in children’s speech: A comparison of boys with and without gender identity disorder. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 137(4), 1995–2003. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.4916202
  • Namy, L. L., Nygaard, L. C., & Sauerteig, D. (2002). Gender differences in vocal accommodation: The role of perception. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(4), 422–432. https://doi.org/10.1177/026192702237958
  • Nardy, A., Chevrot, J.-P., & Barbu, S. (2013). The acquisition of sociolinguistic variation: Looking back and thinking ahead. Linguistics, 51(2), 255–284. https://doi.org/10.1515/ling-2013-0011
  • Nardy, A., Chevrot, J.-P., & Barbu, S. (2014). Sociolinguistic convergence and social interactions within a group of preschoolers: A longitudinal study. Language Variation and Change, 26(3), 273–301. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954394514000131
  • Nielsen, K. (2014). Phonetic imitation by young children and its developmental changes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57(6), 2065–2075. https://doi.org/10.1044/2014_JSLHR-S-13-0093
  • Nycz, J. (2015). Second dialect acquisition: A sociophonetic perspective. Language and Linguistics Compass, 9(11), 469–482. https://doi.org/10.1111/lnc3.12163
  • Olmstead, A. J., Viswanathan, N., Aivar, M. P., & Manuel, S. (2013). Comparison of native and non-native phone imitation by English and Spanish speakers. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(Article), 475. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00475
  • Oviatt, S., Darves, C., & Coulston, R. (2004). Toward adaptive conversation interfaces: Modeling speech convergence with animated personas. ACM Transactions on Computer-human Interaction, 11(3), 300–328. https://doi.org/10.1145/1017494.1017498
  • Papoušek, M., & Papoušek, H. (1989). Forms and functions of vocal matching in interactions between mothers and their precanonical infants. First Language, 9(6), 137–157. https://doi.org/10.1177/014272378900900603
  • Paquette-Smith, M., Schertz, J., & Johnson, E. K. (2021). Comparing phonetic convergence in children and adults. Language and Speech. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177/00238309211013864
  • Pardo, J. S. (2006). On phonetic convergence during conversational interaction. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 119(4), 2382–2393. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.2178720
  • Pardo, J. S., Gibbons, R., Suppes, A., & Krauss, R. M. (2012). Phonetic convergence in college roommates. Journal of Phonetics, 40(1), 190–197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.wocn.2011.10.001
  • Pardo, J. S., Jay, I. C., & Krauss, R. M. (2010). Conversational role influences speech imitation. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72(8), 2254–2264. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196699
  • Pardo, J. S., Jordan, K., Mallari, R., Scanlon, C., & Lewandowski, E. (2013). Phonetic convergence in shadowed speech: The relation between acoustic and perceptual measures. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(3), 183–195. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.06.002
  • Pardo, J. S., Urmanche, A., Wilman, S., & Wiener, J. (2017). Phonetic convergence across multiple measures and model talkers. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 79(2), 637–659. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-016-1226-0
  • Pawlby, S. J. (1977). A study of imitative interaction between mothers and their infants [Doctoral dissertation, University of Nottingham]. Proquest Dissertations Publishing.
  • Payne, A. C. (1976). The Acquisition of the Phonological System of a Second Dialect [Doctoral dissertation, University of Pennsylvania]. Proquest Dissertations Publishing.
  • Perry, T. L., Ohde, R. N., & Ashmead, D. H. (2001). The acoustic bases for gender identification from children’s voices. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 109(6), 2988–2998. https://doi.org/10.1121/1.1370525
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(2), 169–190. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000056
  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–347. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12001495
  • Powell, L. J., & Spelke, E. S. (2018). Human infants’ understanding of social imitation: Inferences of affiliation from third party observations. Cognition, 170, 31–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2017.09.007
  • Rasilo, H., & Räsänen, O. (2017). An online mo+del for vowel imitation learning. Speech Communication, 86, 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2016.10.010
  • Sancier, M. L., & Fowler, C. A. (1997). Gestural drift in a bilingual speaker of Brazilian Portuguese and English. Journal of Phonetics, 25(4), 421–436. https://doi.org/10.1006/jpho.1997.0051
  • Sankoff, G. (2006). Age: Apparent time and real time. In K. Brown Ed., Encyclopedia of language & linguistics (2nd ed., pp. 110–116). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B0-08-044854-2/01479-6
  • Sankoff, G., & Blondeau, H. (2007). Language change across the lifespan: /r/ in Montreal French. Language, 83(3), 560–588. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2007.0106
  • Schertz, J., Paquette-Smith, M., & Johnson, E. K. (2019). The relationship between perceptual similarity judgments and VOT convergence in a shadowing task. In S. Calhoun, P. Escudero, M. Tabain, & P. Warren (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (pp. 3711–3715). Melbourne, Australia: Australasian Speech Science and Technology Association Inc.
  • Seidl, A., Cristia, A., Soderstrom, M., Ko, E.-S., Abel, E. A., Kellerman, A., & Schwichtenberg, A. J. (2018). Infant-mother acoustic-prosodic alignment and developmental risk. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 61(6), 1369–1380. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_JSLHR-S-17-0287
  • Shimpi, P. M., Gámez, P. B., Huttenlocher, J., & Vasilyeva, M. (2007). Syntactic priming in 3- and 4-year-old children: Evidence for abstract representations of transitive and dative forms. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 1334–1346. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1334
  • Shockey, L. (1984). All in a flap: Long-term accommodation in phonology. International Journal of the Sociology of Language, 1984(46), 87–95. https://doi.org/10.1515/ijsl.1984.46.87
  • Shockley, K., Sabadini, L., & Fowler, C. A. (2004). Imitation in shadowing words. Perception & Psychophysics, 66(3), 422–429. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194890
  • Siegel, G. M., Cooper, M., Morgan, J. L., & Brenneise-Sarshad, R. (1990). Imitation of intonation by infants. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 33(1), 9–15. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshr.3301.09
  • Smith, J., & Durham, M. (2019). Sociolinguistic variation in children’s language: Acquiring community norms. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/9781316779248
  • Sonderegger, M., Bane, M., & Graff, P. (2017). The medium-term dynamics of accents on reality television. Language, 93(3), 598–640. https://doi.org/10.1353/lan.2017.0038
  • Stoehr, A., Benders, T., Van Hell, J. G., & Fikkert, P. (2019). Bilingual preschoolers’ speech is associated with non-native maternal language input. Language Learning and Development, 15(1), 75–100. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2018.1533473
  • Story, B. H., & Bunton, K. (2016). Formant measurement in children’s speech based on spectral filtering. Speech Communication, 76, 93–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.specom.2015.11.001
  • Street, R. L., Jr. (1983). Noncontent speech convergence and divergence in adult-child interactions. Annals of the International Communication Association, 7(1), 369–395. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.1983.11678543
  • Sundqvist, A., Nordqvist, E., Koch, F.-S., & Heimann, M. (2016). Early declarative memory predicts productive language: A longitudinal study of deferred imitation and communication at 9 and 16 months. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 151, 109–119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.015
  • Tagliamonte, S. A., & Molfenter, S. (2007). How’d you get that accent?: Acquiring a second dialect of the same language. Language in Society, 36(5), 649–675. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0047404507070911
  • Walker, A., & Campbell-Kibler, K. (2015). Repeat what after whom? Exploring variable selectivity in a cross-dialectal shadowing task. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, Article 546. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00546
  • Welkowitz, J., Cariffe, G., & Feldstein, S. (1976). Conversational congruence as a criterion of socialization in children. Child Development, 47(1), 269–272. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128311
  • Wynn, C. J., Borrie, S. A., & Pope, K. A. (2019). Going with the flow: An examination of entrainment in typically developing children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing, 62(10), 3706–3713. https://doi.org/10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-19-0116
  • Wynn, C. J., Borrie, S. A., & Sellers, T. (2018). Speech rate entrainment in children and adults with and without autism spectrum disorder. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 27(3), 965–974. https://doi.org/10.1044/2018_AJSLP-17-0134

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.