7,484
Views
12
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
General Articles

Theory of mind: The state of the artFootnote*

Pages 728-755 | Received 25 Jan 2018, Accepted 26 Jan 2018, Published online: 15 Feb 2018

References

  • Amsterlaw, J. , & Wellman, H. M. (2006). Theories of mind in transition: A microgenetic study of the development of false belief understanding. Journal of Cognition and Development , 7 (2), 139–172. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_1
  • Aschersleben, G. , Hofer, T. , & Jovanovic, B. (2008). The link between infant attention to goal-directed action and later theory of mind abilities. Developmental Science , 11 (6), 862–868. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00736.x
  • Astington, J. W. , Harris, P. L. , & Olson, D. R. (1988). Developing theories of mind . New York, NY : Cambridge University Press.
  • Baillargeon, R. , Scott, R. M. , & He, Z. (2010). False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 14 (3), 110–118. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006
  • Baron-Cohen, S. , Wheelwright, S. , & Jolliffe, T. (1997). Is there a ‘language of the eyes’? Evidence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition , 4 (3), 311–331. doi:10.1080/713756761
  • Baron-Cohen, S. , Wheelwright, S. , Hill, J. , Raste, Y. , & Plumb, I. (2001). The ‘Reading the mind in the eyes’ Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 42 (2), 241–251. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00715
  • Barrett, J. L. , Richert, R. A. , & Driesenga, A. (2001). God’s beliefs versus mother’s: The development of nonhuman agent concepts. Child Development , 72 , 50–65.
  • Bartsch, K. , & Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind . New York, NY : Oxford University Press.
  • Behne, T. , Carpenter, M. , Call, J. , & Tomasello, M. (2005). Unwilling versus unable: Infants’ understanding of intentional action. Developmental Psychology , 41 (2), 328–337. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.2.328
  • Bering, J. M. , & Bjorklund, D. F. (2004). The natural emergence of reasoning about the afterlife as a developmental regularity. Developmental Psychology , 40 (2), 217–233. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.217
  • Bowman, L. C. , & Wellman, H. M. (2014). Neuroscience contributions to childhood theory-of-mind development. In O. N. Saracho (Ed.), Contemporary perspectives on research in theories of mind in early childhood education (pp. 195–223). Charlotte, NC : Information Age Publishing.
  • Bowman, L. C. , Liu, D. , Meltzoff, A. N. , & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Neural correlates of belief- and desire-reasoning in 7- and 8-year-old children: An event-related potential study. Developmental Science , 15 (5), 618–632. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01158.x
  • Brandone, A. C. , & Wellman, H. M. (2009). You can’t always get what you want: Infants understand failed goal-directed actions. Psychological Science , 20 (1), 85–91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02246.x
  • Brink, K. A. , & Wellman, H. M. (in press). Technology as teacher: How children learn from social robots. In S. Grimm (Ed.), Varieties of understanding: New perspectives from philosophy, psychology and theology . New York, NY : Oxford University Press.
  • Buhs, E. S. , & Ladd, G. W. (2001). Peer rejection as antecedent of young children’s school adjustment: An examination of mediating processes. Developmental Psychology , 37 (4), 550–560. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.37.4.550
  • Buttelmann, D. , Carpenter, M. , & Tomasello, M. (2009). Eighteen-month-old infants show false belief understanding in an active helping paradigm. Cognition , 112 (2), 337–342. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.05.006
  • Churchland, P. M. (1984). Matter and consciousness: A contemporary introduction to the philosophy of mind . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Cutting, A. L. , & Dunn, J. (2002). The cost of understanding other people: Social cognition predicts young children's sensitivity to criticism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 43 (7), 849–860. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.t01-1-00047
  • Devine, R. T. , & Hughes, C. (2013). Silent films and strange stories: Theory of mind, gender, and social experiences in middle childhood. Child Development , 84 (3), 989–1003. doi:10.1111/cdev.12017
  • Dunn, J. (1995). Children as psychologists: The later correlates of individual differences in understanding of emotions and other minds. Cognition & Emotion , 9 , 187–201.10.1080/02699939508409008
  • Elliot, A. J. , & Dweck, C. S. (2005). Competence and motivation: Competence as the core of achievement motivation. In A. J. Elliot , C. S. Dweck , A. J. Elliot , & C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation (pp. 3–12). New York, NY : Guilford Publications.
  • Fodor, J. A. (1987). Psychosemantics: The problem of meaning in the philosophy of mind . Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
  • Gallagher, H. L. , & Frith, C. D. (2003). Functional imaging of ‘theory of mind’. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 7 (2), 77–83. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00025-6
  • Garner, P. W. , & Waajid, B. (2008). The associations of emotion knowledge and teacher-child relationships to preschool children’s school-related developmental competence. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology , 29 (2), 89–100. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2007.12.001
  • Gimínez-Dasí, M. , Guerrero, S. , & Harris, P. L. (2005). Intimations of immortality and omniscience in early childhood. European Journal of Developmental Psychology , 2 (3), 285–297. doi:10.1080/17405620544000039
  • Gopnik, A. , & Wellman, H. M. (1992). Why the child’s theory of mind really is a theory. Mind and Language , 7 , 145–171. doi:10.1111/j.1468-0017.1992.tb00202.x
  • Gopnik, A. , & Wellman, H. M. (1994). The theory theory. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 257–293). New York, NY : Cambridge University Press.10.1017/CBO9780511752902
  • Gopnik, A. , & Wellman, H. M. (2012). Reconstructing constructivism: Causal models, Bayesian learning mechanisms, and the theory theory. Psychological Bulletin , 138 (6), 1085–1108. doi:10.1037/a0028044
  • Happé, F. G. E. (1994). An advanced test of theory of mind: Understanding of story characters’ thoughts and feelings by able autistic, mentally handicapped, and normal children and adults. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders , 24 (2), 129–154. doi:10.1007/BF02172093
  • Hare, B. , & Tomasello, M. (2005). Human-like social skills in dogs? Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 9 (9), 439–444. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.07.003
  • Harris, P. L. , & Giménez, M. (2005). Children’s acceptance of conflicting testimony: The case of death. Journal of Cognition and Culture , 5 , 143–164 . Retrieved from http://booksandjournals.brillonline.com/content/10.1163/1568537054068606 10.1163/1568537054068606
  • Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science , 17 (5), 647–659. doi:10.1111/desc.12148
  • Heyman, G. D. , Dweck, C. S. , & Cain, K. M. (1992). Young children’s vulnerability to self-blame and helplessness: Relationship to beliefs about goodness. Child Development , 63 (2), 401–415. doi:10.2307/1131488
  • Kamins, M. L. , & Dweck, C. S. (1999). Person versus process praise and criticism: Implications for contingent self-worth and coping. Developmental Psychology , 35 (3), 835–847. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.3.835
  • Kristen, S. , Thoermer, C. , Hofer, T. , Aschersleben, G. , & Sodian, B. (2006). Skalierung von “theory of mind” aufgaben (Scaling of theory of mind tasks). Zeitschrift fur Entwicklungspsychologic und Padagogische Psychologie , 38 (4), 186–195. doi:10.1026/0049-8637.38.4.186
  • Kulke, L. , & Rakoczy, H. (2018). Implicit theory of mind – An overview of current replications and non-replications. Data in Brief , 16 (Supplement C), 101–104. doi: 10.1016/j.dib.2017.11.016
  • Lagattuta, K. H. , Hjortsvang, K. , & Kennedy, K. (2014). Theory of mind and academic competence during early childhood: Emotion understanding, relationships, and learning from others. In O. N. Saracho & O. N. Saracho (Eds.), Contemporary perspectives on research in theory of mind in early childhood education (pp. 245–267). Charlotte, NC : IAP Information Age Publishing.
  • Lane, J. D. , Wellman, H. M. , & Evans, E. M. (2012). Sociocultural input facilitates children’s developing understanding of extraordinary minds. Child Development , 83 (3), 1007–1021. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01741.x
  • Lane, J. D. , Wellman, H. M. , & Evans, E. M. (2014). Approaching an understanding of omniscience from the preschool years to early adulthood. Developmental Psychology , 50 (10), 2380–2392. doi:10.1037/a0037715
  • Lane, J. D. , Evans, E. M. , Brink, K. A. , & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Developing concepts of ordinary and extraordinary communication. Developmental Psychology , 52 (1), 19–30. doi:10.1037/dev0000061
  • Lecce, S. , Caputi, M. , & Hughes, C. (2011). Does sensitivity to criticism mediate the relationship between theory of mind and academic achievement? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 110 (3), 313–331. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2011.04.011
  • Lecce, S. , Caputi, M. , & Pagnin, A. (2014). Long-term effect of theory of mind on school achievement: The role of sensitivity to criticism. European Journal of Developmental Psychology , 11 (3), 305–318. doi:10.1080/17405629.2013.821944
  • Lecce, S. , Demicheli, P. , Zocchi, S. , & Palladino, P. (2015). The origins of children’s metamemory: The role of theory of mind. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 131 , 56–72. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2014.11.005
  • Lederberg, A. R. , & Everhart, V. S. (1998). Communication between deaf children and their hearing mothers: The role of language, gesture, and vocalizations. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research , 41 (4), 887–899.10.1044/jslhr.4104.887
  • Leslie, A. M. (2005). Developmental parallels in understanding minds and bodies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences , 9 (10), 459–462. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.002
  • Liu, D. , Wellman, H. M. , Tardif, T. , & Sabbagh, M. A. (2004). A meta-analysis of Chinese theory of mind . Paper presented at the International Congress of Psychology, Beijing, China.
  • Lockl, K. , & Schneider, W. (2007). Knowledge about the mind: Links between theory of mind and later metamemory. Child Development , 78 (1), 148–167. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.00990.x
  • Makris, N. , & Pnevmatikos, D. (2007). Children’s understanding of human and super-natural mind. Cognitive Development , 22 (3), 365–375. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.12.003
  • Markus, H. R. , & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review , 98 , 224–253.10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224
  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology , 31 , 838–850.10.1037/0012-1649.31.5.838
  • Meristo, M. , Morgan, G. , Geraci, A. , Iozzi, L. , Hjelmquist, E. , Surian, L. , & Siegal, M. (2012). Belief attribution in deaf and hearing infants. Developmental Science , 15 (5), 633–640. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01155.x
  • Meristo, M. , Strid, K. , & Hjelmquist, E. (2016). Early conversational environment enables spontaneous belief attribution in deaf children. Cognition , 157 , 139–145. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2016.08.023
  • Milligan, K. , Astington, J. W. , & Dack, L. A. (2007). Language and theory of mind: Meta-analysis of the relation between language ability and false-belief understanding. Child Development , 78 (2), 622–646. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01018.x
  • Moeller, M. P. , & Schick, B. (2006). Relations between maternal input and theory of mind understanding in deaf children. Child Development , 77 , 751–766. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00901.x
  • Morgan, G. , & Kegal, J. (2006). Nicaraguan sign language and theory of mind: The issue of critical periods and abilities. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry , 47 , 811–819. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01621.x
  • Neumann, A. , Thoermer, C. , & Sodian, B. (2008). Belief-based actions anticipation in 18-month-old infants . Paper presented at the International Congress of Psychology, Berlin, Germany.
  • Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The geography of thought: How Asians and westerners think differently–and why . New York, NY : Free Press.
  • O’Reilly, K. , Peterson, C. C. , & Wellman, H. M. (2014). Sarcasm and advanced theory of mind understanding in children and adults with prelingual deafness. Developmental Psychology , 50 (7), 1862–1877. doi:10.1037/a0036654
  • Onishi, K. H. , & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science , 308 (5719), 255–258. doi:10.1126/science.1107621
  • Osterhaus, C. , Koerber, S. , & Sodian, B. (2016). Scaling of advanced theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development , 87 (6), 1971–1991. doi:10.1111/cdev.12566
  • Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Perner, J. , & Wimmer, H. (1985). ‘John thinks that Mary thinks that …’: Attribution of second-order beliefs by 5- to 10-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 39 (3), 437–471. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(85)90051-7
  • Peterson, C. C. (2009). Development of social-cognitive and communication skills in children born deaf. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology , 50 (5), 475–483. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00750.x
  • Peterson, C. C. , & Wellman, H. M. (2009). From fancy to reason: Scaling deaf and hearing children’s understanding of theory of mind and pretence. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 27 , 297–310.
  • Peterson, C. C. , & Wellman, H. M. (in press). Longitudinal theory of mind (ToM) development from preschool to adolescence with and without ToM Delay. Child Development .
  • Peterson, C. C. , Wellman, H. M. , & Liu, D. (2005). Steps in theory-of-mind development for children with deafness or autism. Child Development , 76 (2), 502–517. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2005.00859.x
  • Peterson, C. C. , Slaughter, V. P. , & Paynter, J. (2007). Social maturity and theory of mind in typically developing children and those on the autism spectrum. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry , 48 (12), 1243–1250. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01810.x
  • Peterson, C. C. , Wellman, H. M. , & Slaughter, V. (2012). The mind behind the message: Advancing theory of mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger Syndrome. Child Development , 83 , 469–485.
  • Peterson, C. C. , O’Reilly, K. , & Wellman, H. M. (2016). Deaf and hearing children’s development of theory of mind, peer popularity, and leadership during middle childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology , 149 , 146–158. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2015.11.008
  • Premack, D. , & Woodruff, G. (1978). Chimpanzee problem-solving: A test for comprehension. Science , 202 (4367), 532–535. doi:10.1126/science.705342
  • Pyers, J. E. , & Senghas, A. (2009). Language promotes false-belief understanding: Evidence from learners of a new sign language. Psychological Science , 20 (7), 805–812. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02377.x
  • Rhodes, M. , & Wellman, H. (2013). Constructing a new theory from old ideas and new evidence. Cognitive Science , 37 (3), 592–604. doi:10.1111/cogs.12031
  • Rubin, K. H. , Bukowski, W. M. , & Parker, J. G. (2006). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In N. Eisenberg , W. Damon , R. M. Lerner , N. Eisenberg , W. Damon , & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3, Social, emotional, and personality development (6th ed.) . (pp. 571–645). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Ruffman, T. , Slade, L. , & Crowe, E. (2002). The relation between children’s and mothers? Mental state language and theory-of-mind understanding. Child Development , 73 (3), 734–751. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00435
  • Sabbagh, M. A. , Bowman, L. C. , Evraire, L. E. , & Ito, J. M. B. (2009). Neurodevelopmental correlates of theory of mind in preschool children. Child Development , 80 (4), 1147–1162. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01322.x
  • Saxe, R. , Carey, S. , & Kanwisher, N. (2004). Understanding other minds: Linking developmental psychology and functional neuroimaging. Annual Review of Psychology , 55 , 87–124. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142044
  • Saxe, R. R. , Whitfield-Gabrieli, S. , Scholz, J. , & Pelphrey, K. A. (2009). Brain regions for perceiving and reasoning about other people in school-aged children. Child Development , 80 (4), 1197–1209. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01325.x
  • Schneider, W. (2015). Memory development from early childhood through emerging adulthood . Cham: Springer International Publishing.10.1007/978-3-319-09611-7
  • Scott, R. M. , & Baillargeon, R. (2009). Which penguin is this? Attributing false beliefs about object identity at 18 months. Child Development , 80 (4), 1172–1196.10.1111/cdev.2009.80.issue-4
  • Selcuk, B. , Brink, K. A. , Ekerim, M. , & Wellman, H. M. (in press). Sequence of theory-of-mind acquisition in Turkish children from diverse social backgrounds. Infant and Child Development.
  • Shahaeian, A. , Peterson, C. C. , Slaughter, V. , & Wellman, H. M. (2011). Culture and the sequence of steps in theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology , 47 (5), 1239–1247. doi:10.1037/a0023899
  • Siegal, M. , & Varley, R. (2002). Neural systems involved in ‘theory of mind’. Nature Reviews Neuroscience , 3 (6), 463–471. doi:10.1038/nrn844
  • Slaughter, V. , Imuta, K. , Peterson, C. C. , & Henry, J. D. (2015). Meta-analysis of theory of mind and peer popularity in the preschool and early school years. Child Development , 86 (4), 1159–1174. doi:10.1111/cdev.12372
  • Sodian, B. (2011). Theory of mind in infancy. Child Development Perspectives , 5 , 39–43.
  • Southgate, V. , Senju, A. , & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science , 18 (7), 587–592. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01944.x
  • Southgate, V. , Chevallier, C. , & Csibra, G. (2010). Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others’ referential communication. Developmental Science , 13 (6), 907–912. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00946.x
  • Strasser, K. , & del Río, F. (2014). The role of comprehension monitoring, theory of mind, and vocabulary depth in predicting story comprehension and recall of kindergarten children. Reading Research Quarterly , 49 (2), 169–187.10.1002/rrq.2014.49.issue-2
  • Surian, L. , Caldi, S. , & Sperber, D. (2007). Attribution of beliefs by 13-month-old infants. Psychological Science , 18 (7), 580–586. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01943.x
  • Tardif, T. , & Wellman, H. M. (2000a). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology , 36 (1), 25–43. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.1.25
  • Tardif, T. , & Wellman, H. M. (2000b). Acquisition of mental state language in Mandarin- and Cantonese-speaking children. Developmental Psychology , 36 (1), 25–43.10.1037/0012-1649.36.1.25
  • Thoermer, C. , Sodian, B. , Vuori, M. , Perst, H. , & Kristen, S. (2012). Continuity from an implicit to an explicit understanding of false belief from infancy to preschool age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology , 30 , 172–187.
  • Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Wellman, H. M. (2012). Theory of mind: Better methods, clearer findings, more development. European Journal of Developmental Psychology , 9 (3), 313–330. doi:10.1080/17405629.2012.680297
  • Wellman, H. M. (2014). Making minds: How theory of mind develops . New York, NY : Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199334919.001.0001
  • Wellman, H. M. , & Liu, D. (2004). Scaling of theory-of-mind tasks. Child Development , 75 (2), 523–541. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00691.x
  • Wellman, H. M. , Cross, D. , & Watson, J. (2001). A meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development , 72 (3), 655–684. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304
  • Wellman, H. M. , Phillips, A. T. , Dunphy-Lelii, S. , & LaLonde, N. (2004). Infant social attention predicts preschool social cognition. Developmental Science , 7 (3), 283–288.10.1111/desc.2004.7.issue-3
  • Wellman, H. M. , Fang, F. , Liu, D. , Zhu, L. , & Liu, G. (2006). Scaling of theory-of-mind understandings in chinese children. Psychological Science , 17 (12), 1075–1081. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01830.x
  • Wellman, H. M. , Lopez-Duran, S. , LaBounty, J. , & Hamilton, B. (2008). Infant attention to intentional action predicts preschool theory of mind. Developmental Psychology , 44 (2), 618–623. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.618
  • Wellman, H. M. , Fang, F. , & Peterson, C. C. (2011). Sequential progressions in a theory-of-mind scale: Longitudinal perspectives. Child Development , 82 (3), 780–792. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01583.x
  • Wellman, H. M. , Kushnir, T. , Xu, F. , & Brink, K. A. (2016). Infants use statistical sampling to understand the psychological world. Infancy , 1–9 doi:10.1111/infa.12131.
  • Wentzel, K. R. (2003). School adjustment. In W. M. Reynolds , G. E. Miller , W. M. Reynolds , & G. E. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Educational psychology , Vol. 7 (pp. 235–258). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.
  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition , 69 (1), 1–34. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
  • Woodward, A. (2013). Infant foundations of intentional understanding. In M. R. Banaji , S. A. Gelman , M. R. Banaji , & S. A. Gelman (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 75–80). New York, NY : Oxford University Press.10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199890712.001.0001
  • Woolley, J. D. , Phelps, K. E. , Davis, D. L. , & Mandell, D. J. (1999). Where theories of mind meet magic: The development of children’s beliefs about wishing. Child Development , 70 (3), 571–587. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00042

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.