1,136
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Scepticism or conspiracy? A discourse analysis of anti-lockdown comments to online newspaper articles

ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon, ORCID Icon & ORCID Icon
Pages 482-501 | Received 03 Aug 2022, Accepted 01 Feb 2023, Published online: 10 Mar 2023

References

  • Bavel, J. J. V., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., Crockett, M. J., Crum, A. J., Douglas, K. M., Druckman, J. N., Drury, J., Dube, O., Ellemers, N., Finkel, E. J., Fowler, J. H., Gelfand, M., Han, S., Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(5), 460–471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
  • Bazzanella, C. (2011). Redundancy, repetition, and intensity in discourse. Language Sciences, 33(2), 243–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langsci.2010.10.002
  • BBC News. (2021). Covid: Anti-vax protesters disrupt Milton Keynes NHS test centre. Available from: https://www.bbc.co.uk/news/uk-england-beds-bucks-herts-59826625.amp
  • Billig, M. (1991). Ideology and opinions. Sage.
  • Billig, M. (2021). Rhetorical uses of precise numbers and semi-magical round numbers in political discourse about COVID-19: Examples from the government of the United Kingdom. Discourse & Society, 32(5), 542–558. https://doi.org/10.1177/09579265211013115
  • Coen, S., Meredith, J., Woods, R., & Frenandez, A. (2021). Talk like an expert: The construction of expertise in news comments concerning climate change. Public Understanding of Science, 30(4), 400–416. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963662520981729
  • Coles, B. A. (2018). Internet users’ neutralization of the morality of advertisement blocking. Text & Talk, 38(6), 1860–7330. https://doi.org/10.1515/text-2018-0019
  • Coles, B. A., & West, M. (2016). Weaving the internet together: Imagined communities in newspaper comment threads. Computers in Human Behavior, 60, 44–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.049
  • Douglas, K., & Sutton, R. (2018). Why conspiracy theories matter: A social psychological analysis. European Review of Social Psychology, 29(1), 256–298. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2018.1537428
  • Drew, P. (2003). Precision and exaggeration in interaction. American Sociological Review, 68(6), 917–938. https://doi.org/10.2307/1519751
  • Edwards, D., & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. Sage.
  • Goffman, E. (1981). Forms of talk. University of Pennsylvania Press.
  • Goodman, S. (2017). How to conduct a psychological discourse analysis. Critical Approaches to Discourse Analysis Across Disciplines, 9(2), 142–153.
  • Greene, C. M., & Murphy, G. (2021). Quantifying the effects of fake news on behavior: Evidence from a study of COVID-19 misinformation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 27(4), 773–784. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000371
  • Hadlington, L., Harkin, L. J., Kuss, D., Newman, K., & Ryding, F. C. (2022). Perceptions of fake news, misinformation, and disinformation amid the COVID-19 pandemic: A qualitative exploration. Psychology of Popular Media. Advance Online Publication, 12(1), 40–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000387
  • Inwood, O., & Zappavigna, M. (2022). The ID2020 conspiracy theory in YouTube video comments during COVID-19. In M. Demata, V. Zorzi, & A. Zottola (Eds.), Conspiracy theory discourses. John Benjamin.
  • Johnson, A., & Goodman, S. (2013). Reversing racism and the elite conspiracy: Strategies used by the British national party leader in response to hostile media appearances. Discourse, Context & Media, 2(3), 156–164. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcm.2013.04.006
  • Jonas, E., & Muhlberger, C. (2017). Social cognition, motivation and interaction: How do people respond to threats in social interactions? Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 1577. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01577
  • Jost, J., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339–375. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.129.3.339
  • KhosraviNik, M. (2009). The representation of refugees, asylum seekers and immigrants in British newspapers during the Balkan conflict (1999) and the British general election (2005). Discourse & Society, 20(4), 477–498. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926509104024
  • KhosraviNik, M. (2015). Macro and micro legitimation in discourse on Iran’s nuclear programme: The case of Iranian national newspaper kayhan. Discourse & Society, 26(1), 52–73. https://doi.org/10.1177/0957926514541345
  • Lave, J. (1988). Cognition in practice: Mind, mathematics, and culture in everyday life. Cambridge University Press.
  • Lee, C. (2022). COVID-19 conspiracy theories as affective discourse. In M. Demata, V. Zorzi, & A. Zottola (Eds.), Conspiracy theory discourses (pp. 215–238). John Benjamin.
  • Leudar, I., & Antaki, C. (1996). Backing footing. Theory and Psychology, 6(1). https://doi.org/10.1177/0959354396061003
  • Nurse, M., & Grant, W. (2020). I’ll See It when I believe It: Motivated numeracy in perceptions of climate change risk. Environmental Communication, 14(2), 184–201. https://doi.org/10.1080/17524032.2019.1618364
  • Potter, J., & Edwards, D. (1990). Nigel Lawson's tent: Discourse analysis, attribution theory and the social psychology of fact. European Journal of Social Psychology, 20(5), 405–424. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420200504
  • Reicher, S., & Drury, J. (2021). Pandemic fatigue? How adherence to COVID-19 regulations has been misrepresented and why it matters. BMJ, 372, 137. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.n137
  • Sacks, H. (1975). Everyone Has to Lie. In M. Sanches, & B. Blount (Eds.), Sociocultural dimensions of language Use (pp. 57–79). Academic Press.
  • Sacks, H. (1988). On members’ measurement systems. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 22(1), 45–60. https://doi.org/10.1080/08351818809389297
  • Seymour-Smith, S., Wetherell, M., & Phoenix, A. (2002). ‘'My wife ordered me to come!': A discursive analysis of doctors’ and nurses’ accounts of men's use of general practitioners. Journal of Health Psychology, 7(3), 253–267. https://doi.org/10.1177/1359105302007003220
  • Sharrock, W., & Turner, R. (1978). On a conversational environment for equivocality. In J. Schenkein (Ed.), Studies in the organisation of conversational interaction (pp. 173–197). Academic Press.
  • Toth, C. (2020). Repertoires of vaccine refusal in Romania. Vaccines, 8(4), 757. https://doi.org/10.3390/vaccines8040757
  • van Bavel, J., Baicker, K., Boggio, P. S., Capraro, V., Cichocka, A., Cikara, M., Crockett, M. J., Crum, A. J., Douglas, K. M., Druckman, J. N., Drury, J., Dube, O., Ellemers, N., Finkel, E. J., Fowler, J. H., Gelfand, M., Han, S., Haslam, S. A., Jetten, J., … Willer, R. (2020). Using social and behavioural science to support COVID-19 pandemic response. Nature Human Behavoiur, 4(5), 460–471. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0884-z
  • van Mulukom Valerie, Pummerer Lotte J., Alper Sinan, Bai Hui, Čavojová Vladimíra, Farias Jessica, Kay Cameron S., Lazarevic Ljiljana B., Lobato Emilio J.C., Marinthe Gaëlle, Pavela Banai Irena, Šrol Jakub, Žeželj Iris (2022). Antecedents and consequences of COVID-19 conspiracy beliefs: A systematic review. Social Science and Medicine, 301, 114912. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.114912
  • Vayreda, A., & Antaki, C. (2011). To vaccinate or not?: The disqualification of commercial sources of health advice in an online forum. Communication & Medicine, 8(3), 273–282. https://doi.org/10.1558/cam.v8i3.273