159
Views
5
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Repetition in visual word identification: Benefits and costs

, &
Pages 1986-2009 | Received 20 Jun 2013, Accepted 29 Dec 2013, Published online: 19 Mar 2014

REFERENCES

  • Balota, D. A., & Chumbley, J. I. (1984). Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 340–357. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.10.3.340
  • Bavelier, D., Prasada, S., & Segui, J. (1994). Repetition blindness between words: Nature of the orthographic and phonological representations involved. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(6), 1437–1455. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.20.6.1437
  • Bodner, G. E., & Masson, M. E. J. (1997). Masked repetition priming of words and nonwords: Evidence for a nonlexical basis for priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 37, 268–293. doi:10.1006/jmla.1996.2507
  • Bodner, G. E., & Masson, M. E. J. (2001). Prime validity affects masked repetition priming: Evidence for an episodic resource account of priming. Journal of Memory & Language, 45(4), 616–647. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2791
  • Bond, R., & Andrews, S. (2008). Repetition blindness in sentence contexts: Not just an attribution?. Memory & Cognition, 36(2), 295–313. doi:10.3758/mc.36.2.295
  • Bowers, J. S. (2003). An abstractionst account of masked and long-term priming. New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Burt, J. S. (1994). Identity primes produce facilitation in a colour naming task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 47A(4), 957–1000. doi:10.1080/14640749408401103
  • Burt, J. S. (2002). Why do non-color words interfere with color naming?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 1019–1038. doi:10.1006/jmla.2001.2818
  • Burt, J. S., Mardle, L., & Humphreys, M. S. (1996). Expectancy-based associative and identity priming in pronunciation. Australian Journal of Psychology, 48, 64–74. doi:10.1080/00049539608259508
  • Coltheart, V. (2010). A review of repetition blindness phenomena and theories. In V. Coltheart (Ed.), Tutorials in visual cognition (pp. 187–209). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Davis, M. (1970). Effects of interstimulus interval length and variability on startle-response habituation in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 72(2), 177–192. doi:10.1037/h0029472
  • Ferguson, R., Robidoux, S., & Besner, D. (2009). Reading aloud: Evidence for contextual control over lexical activation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(2), 499–507. doi:10.1037/a0013162
  • Forbach, G. B., Stanners, R. F., & Hochhaus, L. (1974). Repetition and practice effects in a lexical decision task. Memory & Cognition, 2(2), 337–339. doi:10.3758/BF03209005
  • Forster, K. I. (1999). The microgenesis of priming effects in lexical access. Brain and Language. Special Issue: Mental lexicon, 68(1–2), 5–15.
  • Forster, K. I., & Davis, C. (1984). Repetition priming and frequency attenuation in lexical access. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 10, 680–698. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.10.4.680
  • Glanzer, M., & Bowles, N. (1976). Analysis of the word frequency effect in recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 2, 21–31.
  • Grainger, J., Diependaele, K., Spinelli, E., Ferrand, L., & Farioli, F. (2003). Masked repetition and phonological priming within and across modalities. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(6), 1256–1269. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.29.6.1256
  • Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103(3), 518–565. doi:10.1037/0033–295x.103.3.518
  • den Heyer, K., Briand, K., & Smith, L. (1985). Automatic and strategic effects in semantic priming: An examination of Becker's verification model. Memory & Cognition, 13(3), 228–232. doi:10.3758/bf03197685
  • Hochhaus, L., & Marohn, K. M. (1991). Repetition blindness depends on perceptual capture and token individuation failure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 17(2), 422–432. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.17.2.422
  • Howard, S., & Burt, J. S. (2010). Evidence from the attentional blink for different sources of word repetition effects. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal. Oct, 19(1), 125–134. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.09.012
  • Huber, D. E. (2008). Immediate priming and cognitive aftereffects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(2), 324–347. doi:10.1037/0096–3445.137.2.324
  • Huber, D. E., Clark, T. F., Curran, T., & Winkielman, P. (2008). Effects of repetition priming on recognition memory: Testing a perceptual fluency-disfluency model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(6), 1305–1324. doi:10.1037/a0013370
  • Huber, D. E., & O'Reilly, R. C. (2003). Persistence and accommodation in short-term priming and other perceptual paradigms: Temporal segregation through synaptic depression. Cognitive Science: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 27(3), 403–430. doi:10.1016/S0364–0213(03)00012–0
  • Huber, D. E., Shiffrin, R. M., Lyle, K. B., & Quach, R. (2002). Mechanisms of source confusion and discounting in short-term priming 2: Effects of prime similarity and target duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(6), 1120–1136. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.28.6.1120
  • Huber, D. E., Shiffrin, R. M., Lyle, K. B., & Ruys, K. I. (2001). Perception and preference in short-term word priming. Psychological Review, 108(1), 149–182. doi:10.1037/0033–295X.108.1.149
  • Humphreys, G. W., Besner, D., & Quinlan, P. T. (1988). Event perception and the word repetition effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 117, 51–67.
  • Jacoby, L. L., & Whitehouse, K. (1989). An illusion of memory: False recognition influenced by unconscious perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(2), 126–135. doi:10.1037/0096–3445.118.2.126
  • Kahneman, D., Treisman, A., & Gibbs, B. J. (1992). The reviewing of object files: Object-specific integration of information. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 175–219. doi:10.1016/0010–0285(92)90007-o
  • Kanwisher, N. G. (1987). Repetition blindness: Type recognition without token individuation. Cognition, 27(2), 117–143. doi:10.1016/0010–0277(87)90016–3
  • Kanwisher, N. G., & Potter, M. C. (1989). Repetition blindness: The effect of stimulus modality and spatial displacement. Memory & Cognition, 17(2), 117–124.
  • Katz, L., Lee, C. H., Tabor, W., Frost, S. J., Mencl, W. E., Sandak, R., … Pugh, K. R. (2005). Behavioral and neurobiological effects of printed word repetition in lexical decision and naming. Neuropsychologia, 43(14), 2068–2083. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.03.022
  • Keefe, D. E., & Neely, J. H. (1990). Semantic priming in the pronunciation task: The role of prospective prime-generated expectancies. Memory & Cognition, 18(3), 289–298.
  • Kilgarriff, A. (1995, November 3, 1998). BNC database and word frequency lists Retrieved December 5, 2000, from http://www.kilgarriff.co.uk/bnc-readme.html
  • Kinoshita, S. (2006). Additive and interactive effects of word frequency and masked repetition in the lexical decision task. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(4), 668–673.
  • Kirsner, K., & Speelman, C. (1996). Skill acquisition and repetition priming: One principle, many processes? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22, 563–575. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.22.3.563
  • Levy, B. A., Nicholls, A., & Kohen, D. (1993). Repeated readings: Process benefits for good and poor readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 56(3), 303–327. doi:10.1006/jecp.1993.1037
  • Lowe, D. G. (1979). Strategies, context, and the mechanism of response inhibition. Memory & Cognition, 7(5), 382–389.
  • Luo, C. R., & Caramazza, A. (1995). Repetition blindness under minimum memory load: Effects of spatial and temporal proximity and the encoding effectiveness of the first item. Perception & Psychophysics, 57(7), 1053–1064.
  • Marohn, K. M., & Hochhaus, L. (1988). Semantic priming increases and repetition priming decreases apparent stimulus duration. Journal of General Psychology, 115(1), 51–61.
  • McKone, E. (1995). Short-term implicit memory for words and nonwords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 1108–1126. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.21.5.1108
  • McQueen, J. M., & Sereno, J. (2005). Cleaving automatic processes from strategic biases in phonological priming. Memory & Cognition, 33(7), 1185–1209.
  • Milliken, B., Joordens, S., Merikle, P. M., & Seiffert, A. E. (1998). Selective attention: A reevaluation of the implications of negative priming. Psychological Review, 105(2), 203–229.
  • Milliken, B., & Lupianez, J. (2007). Repetition costs in word identification: Evaluating a stimulus--response integration account. Psychological Research/Psychologische Forschung, 71(1), 64–76. doi:10.1007/s00426–005–0036–0
  • Monsell, S. (1991). The nature and locus of word frequency effects in reading. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 148–197). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Morton, J. (1969). Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychological Review, 76, 165–178.
  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. W. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 264–336). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Neill, W. T., Neely, J. H., Hutchison, K. A., Kahan, T. A., & VerWys, C. A. (2002). Repetition blindness, forward and backward. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28(1), 137–149. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.28.1.137
  • Neill, W. T., Valdes, L. A., Terry, K. M., & Gorfein, D. S. (1992). Persistence of negative priming: II. Evidence for episodic trace retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18(5), 993–1000. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.18.5.993
  • Norris, D. (2006). The Bayesian reader: Explaining word recognition as an optimal Bayesian decision process. Psychological Review, 113(2), 327–357. doi:10.1037/0033–295x.113.2.327
  • Norris, D., & Kinoshita, S. (2008). Perception as evidence accumulation and Bayesian inference: Insights from masked priming. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137(3), 434–455. doi:10.1037/a0012799
  • Park, J., & Kanwisher, N. (1994). Determinants of repetition blindness. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(3), 500–519. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.20.3.500
  • Ratcliff, R., Hockley, W., & McKoon, G. (1985). Components of activation: Repetition and priming effects in lexical decision and recognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 114(4), 435–450. doi:10.1037/0096–3445.114.4.435
  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (1997). A counter model for implicit priming in perceptual word identification. Psychological Review, 104(2), 319–343. doi:10.1037/0033–295x.104.2.319
  • Ratcliff, R., & McKoon, G. (2001). A multinomial model for short-term priming in word identification. Psychological Review, 108(4), 835–846. doi:10.1037/0033–295X.108.4.835
  • Robert, C., & Mathey, S. (2005). Effets de distribution du voisinage orthographique et d'amorçage par répétition masqué. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 59(3), 190–198. doi:10.1037/h0087474
  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception. Part 2: The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89, 60–94. doi:10.1037/0033–295X.89.1.60
  • Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., & Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 3, 1–17. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.3.1.1
  • Schacter, D. L., & Buckner, R. L. (1998). On the relations among priming, conscious recollection, and intentional retrieval: Evidence from neuroimaging research. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 70(1–2), 284–303. doi:10.1006/nlme.1998.3854
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolutto, A. (2002). E-Prime user's guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Tools.
  • Segui, J., & Grainger, J. (1990). Priming word recognition with orthographic neighbors: Effects of relative prime-target frequency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 65–76. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.16.1.65
  • Sokolov, E. N. (1990). The orienting response, and future directions of its development. Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science, 25(3), 142–150.
  • Tipper, S. P. (1985). The negative priming effect: Inhibitory priming by ignored objects. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A: Human Experimental Psychology, 37A(4), 571–590.
  • Tweedy, J. R., Lapinski, R. H., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1977). Semantic-context effects on word recognition: Influence of varying the proportion of items presented in an appropriate context. Memory & Cognition, 5(1), 84–89.
  • Versace, R., & Nevers, B. (2003). Word frequency effect on repetition priming as a function of prime duration and delay between the prime and the target. British Journal of Psychology, 94(3), 389–408. doi:10.1348/000712603767876299
  • Weidemann, C. T., Huber, D. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2005). Confusion and compensation in visual perception: Effects of spatiotemporal proximity and selective attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31(1), 40–61. doi:10.1037/0096–1523.31.1.40
  • Weidemann, C. T., Huber, D. E., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2008). Prime diagnosticity in short-term repetition priming: Is primed evidence discounted, even when it reliably indicates the correct answer?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34(2), 257–281. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.34.2.257
  • Whittlesea, B. W. A., & Masson, M. E. J. (2005). Repetition blindness in rapid lists: Activation and inhibition versus construction and attribution. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(1), 54–67. doi:10.1037/0278–7393.31.1.54
  • Wong, K. F. E., & Chen, H.-C. (2009). Forward and backward repetition blindness in speed and accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35(3), 778–786. doi:10.1037/a0013898
  • Wyble, B., Potter, M. C., Bowman, H., & Nieuwenstein, M. (2011). Attentional episodes in visual perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(3), 488–505. doi:10.1037/a0023612

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.