190
Views
39
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

On the advantages of word frequency and contextual diversity measures extracted from subtitles: The case of Portuguese

, , , , , , & show all
Pages 680-696 | Received 10 Apr 2014, Accepted 10 Jun 2014, Published online: 07 Nov 2014

REFERENCES

  • Adelman, J. S., Brown, G. D. A., & Quesada, J. F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17, 814–823. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01787.x
  • Alegria, J., Marin, J., Carrillo, S., & Mousty, P. (2003). Les premiers pas dans l'acquisition de l'orthographe en function du caractère profound ou superficial du système alphabétique: Comparaison entre le français et l'espagnol. In M. N. Romdhane, J.-E. Gombert, & M. Belajouza (Eds.), L'apprentissage de la lecture: Perspectives comparatives (pp. 51–67). Rennes: Presses Universitaires de Rennes.
  • Baayen, R. H. (2011). Corpus linguistics and naive discriminative learning. Brazilian Journal of Applied Linguistics, 11, 295–328.
  • Baayen, R. H., Feldman, L. B., & Schreuder, R. (2006). Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 53, 496–512.
  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & Gulikers, L. (1993). The CELEX lexical database. Philadelphia: Linguistic Data Consortium, University of Pennsylvania.
  • Bacelar do Nascimento, M. F., Pereira, L. A. S., & Saramago, J. (2000). Portuguese Corpora at CLUL. In Proceedings of the Second International Conference on Language Resources and Evaluation (pp. 1603–1607). Athens, Greece.
  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283
  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Hutchison, K. A., Cortese, M. J., Kessler, B., Loftis, B., … Treiman, R. (2007). The english lexicon project. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 445–459. doi: 10.3758/BF03193014
  • Bonin, P., Chalard, M., Méot, A., & Fayol, M. (2001). Age-of-acquisition and word frequency in the lexical decision task: Further evidence from the French language. Current Psychology of Cognition, 20, 401–443.
  • Breland, H. M. (1996). Word frequency and word difficulty: A comparison of counts in four corpora. Psychological Science, 7, 96–99. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00336.x
  • Brysbaert, M., Buchmeier, M., Conrad, M., Jacobs, A. M., Bölte, J., & Böhl, A. (2011). The word frequency effect. Experimental Psychology, 58, 412–424. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169/a000123
  • Brysbaert, M., & Cortese, M. J. (2011). Do the effects of subjective frequency and age of acquisition survive better word frequency norms?. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 64, 545–559. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2010.503374
  • Brysbaert, M., & Diependaele, K. (2013). Dealing with zero word frequencies: A review of the existing rules of thumb and a suggestion for an evidence-based choice. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 422–430. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0270-5
  • Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., & New, B. (2011). Assessing the usefulness of Google Books’ word frequencies for psycholinguistic research on word processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 27. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00027
  • Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2009). Moving beyond Kučera and Francis: A critical evaluation of current word frequency norms and the introduction of a new and improved word frequency measure for American English. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 977–990. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.977
  • Brysbaert, M., New, B., & Keuleers, E. (2012). Adding part-of-speech information to the SUBTLEX-US word frequencies. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 991–997. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0190-4
  • Burgess, C., & Livesay, K. (1998). The effect of corpus size in predicting reaction time in a basic word recognition task: Moving on from Kučera and Francis. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 30, 272–277. doi: 10.3758/BF03200655
  • Cai, Q., & Brysbaert, M. (2010). SUBTLEX-CH: Chinese word and character frequencies based on film subtitles. PLoS ONE, 5, e10729. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010729
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
  • Comesaña, M., Fraga, I., Moreia, A. J., Frade, C. S., & Soares, A. P. (2014). Free associate norms for 139 European Portuguese words for children from different age groups. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 564–574. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0388-0
  • Cortese, M. J., & Khanna, M. M. (2007). Age of acquisition predicts naming and lexical-decision performance above and beyond 22 other predictor variables: An analysis of 2,342 words. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 1072–1082.
  • Cuetos, F., & Barbón, A. (2006). Word naming in Spanish. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 18, 415–436. doi: 10.1080/13594320500165896
  • Cuetos, F., Ellis, A. W., & Alvarez, B. (1999). Naming times for the Snodgrass and Vanderwart pictures in Spanish. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 650–658. doi: 10.3758/BF03200741
  • Cuetos, F., Glez-Nosti, M., Barbon, A., & Brysbaert, M. (2011). SUBTLEX-ESP: Spanish word frequencies based on film subtitles. Psicologica, 32, 133–143.
  • Davis, C. J. (2010). The spatial coding model of visual word identification. Psychological Review, 117, 713–758. doi: 10.1037/a0019738
  • Dimitropoulou, M., Duñabeitia, J. A., Avilés, A., Corral, J., & Carreiras, M. (2010). Subtitle-based word frequencies as the best estimate of reading behavior: The case of Greek. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1–12. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00218
  • Duchon, A., Perea, M., Sebastián-Gallés, N., Martí, A., Carreiras, M. (2013). EsPal: One-stop shopping for Spanish word properties. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1246–1258. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0326-1
  • Engbert, R., Nuthmann, A., Richter, E., & Kliegl, R. (2005). SWIFT: A dynamical model of saccade generation during reading. Psychological Review, 112, 777–813. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.777
  • Equipe DELIC. (2004). Présentation du Corpus de référence du français parle [Presentation of the Reference Corpus of Spoken French]. Recherches sur le français parlé, 18, 11–42. Also available at http://www.up.univ-mrs.fr/veronis/pdf/2004-presentation-crfp.pdf
  • European Social Survey. (2008). ESS4-European Social Survey 2002/2008. Available at www.europeansocialsurvey.org/
  • Ferrand, L., New, B., Brysbaert, M., Keuleers, E., Bonin, P., …  Pallier, C. (2010). The French lexicon project: Lexical decision data for 38,840 French words and 38,840 pseudowords. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 488–496. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.2.488
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 116–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03195503
  • Freitas, E., Casanova, J. L., & Alves, N. A. (1997). Hábitos de leitura: Um inquérito à população portuguesa [Reading habits: A Portuguese population survey]. Lisboa: Dom Quixote.
  • Frota, S., Vigário, M., & Martins, F. (2002). Language discrimination and rhythm classes: Evidence from Portuguese. In B. Bel & I. Marlien (Eds.), Proceedings of the 1st International Conference on Speech Prosody (pp. 315–318). Université de Provence: Laboratoire de Parole et Language: Aix-en-Provence, France.
  • Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & de Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children's orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: Nonsense word reading in English, French and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 19–52. doi: 10.1017/S0142716400010560
  • Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103, 518–565. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.518
  • Howes, D. H., & Solomon, R. L. (1951). Visual duration threshold as a function of word-probability. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 41, 401–410. doi: 10.1037/h0056020
  • IMBD website. Retrieved May 3, 2014, from http://www.imdb.com/genre/
  • Johns, B. T., Gruenenfelder, T. M., Pisoni, D. B., & Jones, M. N. (2012). Effects of word frequency, contextual diversity, and semantic distinctiveness on spoken word recognition. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 132, EL74–EL80. doi: 10.1121/1.4731641
  • Keuleers, E., Brysbaert, M., & New, B. (2010). SUBTLEX-NL: A new measure for Dutch words frequency based on film subtitles. Behavior Research Methods, 42, 643–650. doi: 10.3758/BRM.42.3.643
  • Keuleers, E., Diependaele, K. & Brysbaert, M. (2010). Practice effects in large-scale visual word recognition studies: A lexical decision study on 14,000 Dutch mono- and disyllabic words and nonwords. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 174. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00174
  • Keuleers, E., Lacey, P., Rastle, K., & Brysbaert, M. (2012). The British Lexicon Project: Lexical decision data for 28,730 monosyllabic and disyllabic English words. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 287–304. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0118-4
  • Kučera, M., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University Press.
  • Lee, C. J. (2003). Evidence-based selection of word frequency lists. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 27, 172–175.
  • Leech, G., Rayson, P., & Wilson, A. (2001). Word frequencies in written and spoken English: Based on the British National Corpus. London: Longman.
  • Lund, K., & Burgess, C. (1996). Producing high-dimensional semantic spaces from lexical co-occurrence. Behavior Research Methods, Instrumentation, & Computers, 28, 203–208. doi: 10.3758/BF03204766
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. An account of Basic Findings. Psychological Review, 88, 375–407. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375
  • Michel, J. B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., Pickett, J. P., … Aiden, E. L. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331, 176–182. doi: 10.1126/science.1199644
  • Murray, W. S., & Forster, K. I. (2004). Serial mechanisms in lexical access: The rank hypothesis. Psychological Review, 111, 721–756. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.111.3.721
  • National Endowment for the Arts (NEA). (2004). Reading at risk: A survey of literary reading in America. Washington, DC: National Endowment for the Arts. NW.
  • New, B., Brysbaert, M., Veronis, J., & Pallier, C. (2007). The use of film subtitles to estimate word frequencies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 661–677. doi: 10.1017/S014271640707035X
  • New, B., Pallier, C., Brysbaert, M., & Ferrand, L. (2004). Lexique 2: A new French lexical database. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 516–524. doi: 10.3758/BF03195598
  • Open Subtitles (OS) website. Retrieved May 3, 2014, from http://opus.lingfil.uu.se/OpenSubtitles_v2.php
  • Perea, M., Soares, A. P., & Comesaña, M. (2013). Contextual diversity is a main determinant of word identification times in young readers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 37–44. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.10.014
  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.56
  • Plummer, P., Perea, M., & Rayner, K. (2014). The influence of contextual diversity on eye movements in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 275–283.
  • Procura-PALavras database [P-PAL] website. Retrieved May 3, 2014, from http://p-pal.di.uminho.pt/tools
  • Reichle, E. D., Pollatsek, A., Fisher, D. L., & Rayner, K. (1998). Toward a model of eye movement control in reading. Psychological Review, 105, 125–157. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.105.1.125
  • Santos, M. L., Neves, J., Lima, M. J., & Carvalho, M. (2007). A leitura em Portugal [Reading in Portugal]. Lisboa: Gabinete de Estatística e Planeamento da Educação (GEPE).
  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174. doi: 10.1348/000712603321661859
  • Simões, A. M., & Almeida, J. J. (2001). Jspell. In Actas do Encontro Nacional da Associação Portuguesa de Linguística. Lisboa: Associação Portuguesa de Linguística.
  • Sinclair, J. (2005). Corpus and text: Basic principles. In M. Wynne (Ed.), Developing linguistic corpora: A guide to good practice (pp. 1–16). Oxford, UK: Oxbow Books.
  • Soares, A. P., Comesaña, M., Pinheiro, A. P., Simões, A., & Frade, C. S. (2012). The adaptation of the affective norms for english words (ANEW) for European Portuguese. Behavior Research Methods, 44, 256–269. doi: 10.3758/s13428-011-0131-7
  • Soares, A. P., Costa, A., Machado, J., Silva, A., Oliveira, J., Gonçalves, A. M., & Comesaña, M. (2013). Subjective frequency, imageability and concreteness norms for 3,800 European Portuguese words. Poster presented at 18th Conference of the European Society for Cognitive Psychology (ESCOP), 29 August-01 September, Budapest, Hungary.
  • Soares, A. P., Iriarte, A., Almeida, J. J., Simões, A., Costa, A., França, P., …  Comesaña, M. (2014). Procura-PALavras (P-PAL): Uma nova medida de frequência lexical do Português Europeu contemporâneo [Procura-PALavras (P-PAL): A new measure of word frequency for contemporary European Portuguese]. Psicologia: Reflexão e Crítica, 27, 110–123.
  • Soares, A. P., Medeiros, J. C., Simões, A., Machado, J., Costa, A., Iriarte, A., …  Comesaña, M. (2014). ESCOLEX: A grade-level lexical database from European Portuguese Elementary to Middle School textbooks. Behavior Research Methods, 46, 240–253. doi: 10.3758/s13428-013-0350-1
  • Soares, A. P., Pinheiro, A. P., Costa, A., Frade, C. S., Comesaña, M., & Pureza, R. (2013). Affective auditory stimuli: Adaptation of the international affective digitized sounds (IADS-2) for European Portuguese. Behavior Research Methods, 45, 1168–1181. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0310-1
  • SUBTLEX-PT database website. Retrieved May 3, 2014, from http://p-pal.di.uminho.pt/about/databases
  • Thorndike, E. L. (1921). The teacher's word book. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.
  • Thorndike, E. L., & Lorge, I. (1944). The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University.
  • Tiedemann, J. (2009). News from OPUS: A collection of multilingual parallel corpora with tools and interfaces. In N. Nicolov, K. Bontcheva, G. Angelova & R. Mitkov (Eds.), Recent advances in natural language processing (pp. 237–248). Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins.
  • Universidade de Lisboa. (1987). Português fundamental: Métodos e documentos [Fundamental Portuguese: Methods and documents]. Lisboa: Instituto de Investigação Científica.
  • van Heuven, W. J. B., Mandera, P., Keuleers, E., & Brysbaert, M. (2014). SUBTLEX-UK: A new and improved word frequency database for British English. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67, 1176–1190. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.850521
  • Yap, M. J., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 502–529. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.02.001
  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educators word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science.
  • Zevin, J. D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2002). Age of acquisition effects in word reading and other tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 1–29. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2001.2834
  • Ziegler, J. C., Petrova, A., & Ferrand, L. (2008). Feedback consistency effects in visual and auditory word recognition: Where do we stand after more than a decade?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 34, 643–661.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.