331
Views
16
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Default spacing is the optimal spacing for word reading

&
Pages 697-709 | Received 04 Sep 2014, Accepted 06 Sep 2014, Published online: 07 Nov 2014

REFERENCES

  • Baayen, R. H., Piepenbrock, R., & van Rijn, H. (1993). The CELEX lexical database [CD-ROM]. Philadelphia, PA: University of Pennsylvania, Linguistic Data Consortium.
  • Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., Sergent-Marshall, S. D., Spieler, D. H., & Yap, M. J. (2004). Visual word recognition of single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.133.2.283
  • Bouma, H. (1970). Interaction effects in parafoveal letter recognition. Nature, 226, 177–178. doi:10.1038/226177a0
  • Brus, B., & Voeten, B. (1995). Eén minuut test vorm A en B. Verantwoording en handleiding [one-minute-test manual]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swets & Zietlinger.
  • Callens, M., Whitney, C., Tops, W., & Brysbaert, M. (2013). No deficiency in left-to-right processing of words in dyslexia but evidence for enhanced visual crowding. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 1803–1817. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.766898
  • Chung, S. T. L. (2002). The effect of letter spacing on reading speed in central and peripheral vision. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, 43, 1270–1276.
  • Cohen, L., Dehaene, S., Vinckier, F., Jobert, A., & Montavont, A. (2008). Reading normal and degraded words: Contribution of the dorsal and ventral visual pathways. NeuroImage, 40, 353–366. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.036
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
  • Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. TRENDS in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 335–341. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004
  • Legge, G. E., & Bigelow, C. A. (2011). Does print size matter for reading? A review of findings from vision science and typography. Journal of Vision, 11, 1–22. doi:10.1167/11.5.8
  • Martelli, M., Di Filippo, G., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2009). Crowding, reading, and developmental dyslexia. Journal of Vision, 9(4), 14, 1–18. doi:10.1167/9.4.14
  • Marzouki, Y., & Grainger, J. (2014). Effects of stimulus duration and inter-letter spacing on letter-in-string identification. Acta Psychologica, 148, 49–55. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.12.011
  • Moll, K., & Jones, M. (2013). Naming fluency in dyslexic and nondyslexic readers: Differential effects of visual crowding in foveal, parafoveal, and peripheral vision. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 2085–2091. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.840852
  • Paterson, K. B., & Jordan, T. R. (2010). Effects of increased letter spacing on word identification and eye guidance during reading. Memory & Cognition, 38, 502–512. doi:10.3758/MC.38.4.502
  • Perea, M., & Gomez, P. (2012). Subtle increases in interletter spacing facilitate the encoding of words during normal reading. PLOSone, 7, e47568. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047568
  • Perea, M., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2011). The effect of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition. Acta Psychologica, 137, 345–351. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.04.003
  • Perea, M., Panadero, V., Moret-Tatay, C., & Gómez, P. (2012). The effects of inter-letter spacing in visual-word recognition: Evidence with young normal readers and developmental dyslexics. Learning and Instruction, 22, 420–430. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.04.001
  • Quené, H., & van den Bergh, H. (2004). On multi-level modeling of data from repeated measures designs: A tutorial. Speech Communication, 43, 103–121. doi:10.1016/j.specom.2004.02.004
  • Rasbash, J., Steele, F., Browne, W. J., & Goldstein, H. (2008). A User's guide to MLwiN version 2.10. Bristol: Centre for Multilevel Modelling, University of Bristol.
  • Reynolds, L., & Walker, S. (2004). “You can't see what the words say”: Word spacing and letter spacing in children's reading books. Journal of Research in Reading, 27, 87–98. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00216.x
  • Risko, E. F., Lanthier, S. N., & Besner, D. (2011). Basic processes in reading: The effect of interletter spacing. Journal of Experimental Pscyhology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1449–1457. doi:10.1037/a0024332
  • Schneider, W., Eschman, A., & Zuccolotto, A. (2002). E-prime: User's guide. Pittsburgh, PA: Psychology Software Inc.
  • Schneps, M. H., Thomson, J. M., Sonnert, G., Pomplun, M., Chen, C., & Heffner-Wong, A. (2013). Shorter lines facilitate reading in those who struggle. PLoS One, 8, e71161. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0071161
  • Slattery, T. J., & Rayner, K. (2013). Effects of intraword and interword spacing on eye movements during reading: Exploring the optimal use of space in a line of text. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 75, 1275–1292. doi:10.3758/s13414-013-0463-8
  • Spinelli, D., de Luca, M., Judica, A., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Crowding effects on word identification in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 38, 179–200. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70649-X
  • Tai, Y. C., Sheedy, J. E., & Hayes, J. (2006). Effect of letter spacing on eye movements and reading performance. Proceedings of the 2006 symposium on Eye tracking research & applications, San Diego, California, 27–29 March 2006.
  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades?. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40. doi:10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x
  • Vinckier, F., Qiao, E., Pallier, C., Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2011). The impact of letter spacing on reading: A test of the bigram coding hypothesis. Journal of Vision, 11, 1–21. doi:10.1167/11.6.8
  • Yu, D., Cheung, S. H., Legge, G. E., & Chung, S. T. L. (2007). Effect of letter spacing on visual span and reading speed. Journal of Vision, 7, 1–10. doi:10.1167/7.2.2
  • Zorzi, M., Barbiero, C., Facoetti, A., Lonciari, L., Carrozzi, M., Montico, M., … , Ziegler, J. C. (2012). Extra-large letter spacing improves reading in dyslexia. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109, 11455–11459. doi:10.1073/pnas.1205566109

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.