790
Views
18
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
EPS Prize Lecture

Cognitive underpinnings of social interaction

Pages 417-432 | Received 20 Nov 2013, Accepted 10 Jul 2014, Published online: 18 Nov 2014

REFERENCES

  • Aldridge, M. A., Stone, K. R., Sweeney, M. H., & Bower, T. G. R. (2000). Preverbal children with autism understand the intentions of others. Developmental Science, 3(3), 294–301. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00123
  • Amodio, D. M., & Frith, C. (2006). Meeting of minds: The medial frontal cortex and social cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 7(4), 268–277. doi:10.1038/nrn1884
  • Bargh, J., & Dijksterhuis, a. (2001). The perception-behavior expressway: Automatic effects of social perception on social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 1–40. doi:10.1016/S0065-2601(01)80003-4
  • Bargh, J., Schwader, K. L., Hailey, S. E., Dyer, R. L., & Boothby, E. J. (2012). Automaticity in social-cognitive processes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(12), 593–605. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.002
  • Bonini, L., Serventi, F. U., Simone, L., Rozzi, S., Ferrari, P. F., & Fogassi, L. (2011). Grasping neurons of monkey parietal and premotor cortices encode action goals at distinct levels of abstraction during complex action sequences. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(15), 5876–5886. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5186-10.2011
  • Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 343–352. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.008
  • Brass, M., Bekkering, H., & Prinz, W. (2001). Movement observation affects movement execution in a simple response task. Acta Psychologica, 106(1), 3–22.
  • Brass, M., Derrfuss, J., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2005). The inhibition of imitative and overlearned responses: A functional double dissociation. Neuropsychologia, 43(1), 89–98. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.018
  • Brass, M., & Heyes, C. (2005). Imitation: is cognitive neuroscience solving the correspondence problem? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(10), 489–495. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.007
  • Brass, M., Schmitt, R. M., Spengler, S., & Gergely, G. (2007). Investigating action understanding: Inferential processes versus action simulation. Current Biology : CB, 17(24), 2117–2121. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2007.11.057
  • Brochier, T., Spinks, R., Umilta, M. A., Kirkwood, P. A., & Lemon, R. N. (2001). Specificity of muscular activation during skilled grasp of different objects in monkeys.
  • Buxbaum, L. J., & Kalénine, S. (2010). Action knowledge, visuomotor activation, and embodiment in the two action systems. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 201–218. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05447.x
  • Carpenter, M., Pennington, B. F., & Rogers, S. J. (2001). Understanding of others’ intentions in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(6), 589–599. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/11814270 doi: 10.1023/A:1013251112392
  • Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology, 17(17), 1527–1531. doi: S0960-9822(07)01782-4 [pii] 10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.006
  • Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. a. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10402679 doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893
  • Cisek, P., & Kalaska, J. F. (2010). Neural mechanisms for interacting with a world full of action choices. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 33, 269–298. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.051508.135409
  • Cook, J. L., & Bird, G. (2012). Atypical social modulation of imitation in autism spectrum conditions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(6), 1045–1051. doi:10.1007/s10803-011-1341-7
  • Dapretto, M., Davies, M. S., Pfeifer, J. H., Scott, A. A., Sigman, M., Bookheimer, S. Y., & Iacoboni, M. (2006). Understanding emotions in others: Mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders. Nature Neuroscience, 9(1), 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611
  • De Lange, F. P., Spronk, M., Willems, R. M., Toni, I., & Bekkering, H. (2008). Complementary systems for understanding action intentions. Current Biology : CB, 18(6), 454–457. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.057
  • Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Humphreys, K., Minshew, N. J., Behrmann, M., & Heeger, D. J. (2010). Normal movement selectivity in autism. Neuron, 66(3), 461–469. doi: S0896-6273(10)00237-0 [pii] 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.034
  • Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C.-L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It's all in the mind, but whose mind? PloS One, 7(1), e29081. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0029081
  • Edwards, L. A. (2014). A meta-analysis of imitation abilities in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Autism Research, n/a–n/a. doi:10.1002/aur.1379
  • Ferrari, P. F., Visalberghi, E., Paukner, A., Fogassi, L., Ruggiero, A., & Suomi, S. J. (2006). Neonatal imitation in rhesus macaques. PLoS Biology, 4(9), e302. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0040302
  • Fogassi, L., Ferrari, P. F., Gesierich, B., Rozzi, S., Chersi, F., & Rizzolatti, G. (2005). Parietal lobe: From action organization to intention understanding. Science (New York, N.Y.), 308(5722), 662–667. doi:10.1126/science.1106138
  • Frith, C., & Frith, U. (1999). Interacting minds--a biological basis. Science, 286(5445), 1692–1695. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/10576727 doi: 10.1126/science.286.5445.1692
  • Gowen, E. (2012). Imitation in autism: Why action kinematics matter. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 6(December), 117. doi:10.3389/fnint.2012.00117
  • Grafton, S. T., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2007). Evidence for a distributed hierarchy of action representation in the brain. Human Movement Science, 26(4), 590–616. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.05.009
  • Grecucci, A., Brambilla, P., Siugzdaite, R., Londero, D., Fabbro, F., & Rumiati, R. I. (2013). Emotional resonance deficits in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(3), 616–628. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1603-z
  • Grèzes, J., Wicker, B., Berthoz, S., & de Gelder, B. (2009). A failure to grasp the affective meaning of actions in autism spectrum disorder subjects. Neuropsychologia, 47(8–9), 1816–1825. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.02.021
  • Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2008). Emulation and mimicry for social interaction: A theoretical approach to imitation in autism. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (2006), 61(1), 101–115. doi:10.1080/17470210701508798
  • Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2009). Research review: Goals, intentions and mental states: Challenges for theories of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines, 50(8), 881–892. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02098.x
  • Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(max 6), 91–105. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.09.008
  • Hamilton, A. F. de C., Brindley, R., & Frith, U. (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1859–1868. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.022
  • Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Grafton, S. T. (2006). Goal representation in human anterior intraparietal sulcus. Journal of Neuroscience, 26(4), 1133–1137. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4551-05.2006
  • Hamilton, A. F. de C., & Grafton, S. T. (2007). The motor hierarchy: From kinematics to goals and intentions. In P. Haggard, Y. Rosetti, & M. Kawato (Eds.), Attention and Performance xxii. Y. Rosetti, M. Kawato (pp. 1–29). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Retrieved from http://scholar.google.com/scholar?q=intitle:The+motor+hierarchy:+from+kinematics+to+goals+and+intentions#0
  • Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557–559. doi:10.1038/nature06288
  • Heerey, E. a, & Crossley, H. M. (2013). Predictive and reactive mechanisms in smile reciprocity. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1446–1455. doi:10.1177/0956797612472203
  • Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 5(6), 253–261. doi: S1364-6613(00)01661-2[pii] 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01661-2
  • Heyes, C. (2009). Evolution, development and intentional control of imitation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2293–2298. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0049
  • Heyes, C. (2011). Automatic imitation. Psychological Bulletin, 137(3), 463–483. doi:10.1037/a0022288
  • Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(4), 649–659. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00481
  • Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 164–181. doi:10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6
  • Ingersoll, B. R. (2008). The effect of context on imitation skills in children with autism. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 2(2), 332–340. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2007.08.003
  • Ingersoll, B. R., Schreibman, L., & Tran, Q. H. (2003). Effect of sensory feedback on immediate object imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(6), 673–683. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?cmd=Retrieve&db=PubMed&dopt=Citation&list_uids=14714935 doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000006003.26667.f8
  • Jacob, P., & Jeannerod, M. (2005). The motor theory of social cognition: A critique. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 21–25. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.11.003
  • Jastorff, J., Begliomini, C., Fabbri-Destro, M., Rizzolatti, G., & Orban, G. A. (2010). Coding observed motor acts: Different organizational principles in the parietal and premotor cortex of humans. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(1), 128–140. doi:10.1152/jn.00254.2010
  • Jiménez, L., Lorda, M. J., & Méndez, C. (2013). Emulation and mimicry in school students with typical development and with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-2027-0
  • Johnston, L. (2002). Behavioral mimicry and stigmatization. Social Cognition, 20(1), 18–35. doi:10.1521/soco.20.1.18.20944
  • Kilner, J. M., Neal, A., Weiskopf, N., Friston, K. J., & Frith, C. (2009). Evidence of mirror neurons in human inferior frontal gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(32), 10153–10159. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2668-09.2009
  • Kraskov, A., Dancause, N., Quallo, M. M., Shepherd, S., & Lemon, R. N. (2009). Corticospinal neurons in macaque ventral premotor cortex with mirror properties: A potential mechanism for action suppression? Neuron, 64(6), 922–930. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.010
  • Lakin, J. L., Jefferis, V. E., Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). The chameleon effect as social glue: Evidence for the evolutionary significance of nonconscious mimicry. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 27(3), 145–162. doi: 10.1023/A:1025389814290
  • Leighton, J., Bird, G., Orsini, C., & Heyes, C. (2009). Social attitudes modulate automatic imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(April), 905–910. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.04.014
  • Marsh, L., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011). Dissociation of mirroring and mentalising systems in autism. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1511–1519. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.003
  • Marsh, L., Pearson, a, Ropar, D., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2013). Children with autism do not overimitate. Current Biology, 23(29), 6–7. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.036
  • Marsh, L., Ropar, D., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2014). The social modulation of imitation fidelity in school-age children. PLoS ONE, 9(1), e86127. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0086127
  • Marsh, L. E., Mullett, T. L., Ropar, D., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2014). Responses to irrational actions in action observation and mentalising networks of the human brain. NeuroImage.
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science (New York, N.Y.), 198(4312), 75–78. doi:10.1126/science.198.4312.75
  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1997). Explaining facial imitation: A theoretical model. Early Development & Parenting, 6(3-4), 179.
  • Nielsen, M., Slaughter, V., & Dissanayake, C. (2013). Object-directed imitation in children with high-functioning autism: Testing the social motivation hypothesis. Autism Research : Official Journal of the International Society for Autism Research, 6(1), 23–32. doi:10.1002/aur.1261
  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour. In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Vol. 4. Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). New York: Plenum Press.
  • O'Connell, G., Christakou, A., Haffey, A. T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2013). The role of empathy in choosing rewards from another's perspective. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(May), 174. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00174
  • Oosterhof, N. N., Wiggett, A. J., Diedrichsen, J., Tipper, S. P., & Downing, P. E. (2010). Surface-based information mapping reveals crossmodal vision-action representations in human parietal and occipitotemporal cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(2), 1077–1089. doi:10.1152/jn.00326.2010
  • Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Developmental Science, 12(3), F1–F8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.x
  • Pobric, G., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2006). Action understanding requires the left inferior frontal cortex. Current Biology, 16(5), 524–529. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.01.033
  • Ramsey, R., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2010). Triangles have goals too: Understanding action representation in left aIPS. Neuropsychologia, 48(9), 2773–2776. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.04.028
  • Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(4), 264–274. doi: nrn2805 [pii] 10.1038/nrn2805
  • Rothi, L. J., Ochipa, C., Heilman, K. M., & Gonzalez Rothi, L. J. (1991). A cognitive neuropsychological model of limb praxis. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 8(6), 443–458. doi:10.1080/02643299108253382
  • Rumiati, R. I., Papeo, L., & Corradi-Dell'Acqua, C. (2010). Higher-level motor processes. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1191(1), 219–241. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05442.x
  • Sasaki, A. T., Kochiyama, T., Sugiura, M., Tanabe, H. C., & Sadato, N. (2012). Neural networks for action representation: A functional magnetic-resonance imaging and dynamic causal modeling study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(August), 236. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00236
  • Sims, T. B., Van Reekum, C. M., Johnstone, T., & Chakrabarti, B. (2012). How reward modulates mimicry: EMG evidence of greater facial mimicry of more rewarding happy faces. Psychophysiology, 49(7), 998–1004. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01377.x
  • Spunt, R. P., Falk, E. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2010). Dissociable neural systems support retrieval of how and why action knowledge. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1593–1598. doi:10.1177/0956797610386618
  • Subiaul, F. (2010). Dissecting the imitation faculty: The multiple imitation mechanisms (MIM) hypothesis. Behavioural Processes, 83(2), 222–234. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2009.12.002
  • Tomasello, M. (1990). Cultural transmission in the tool use and communicatary signaling of chimpanzees? In S. Parker & K. Gibson (Eds.), Language and Intelligence of Monkeys and Apes: Comparative developmental perspectives. Cambridge: University Press.
  • Uzgiris, I. C. (1981). Two functions of imitation during infancy. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 4(1), 1–12. doi:10.1177/016502548100400101
  • Van Leeuwen, M. L., Veling, H., van Baaren, R. B., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). The influence of facial attractiveness on imitation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(6), 1295–1298. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2009.07.008
  • Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2012). Social top-down response modulation (STORM): A model of the control of mimicry in social interaction. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6(June), 1–10. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00153
  • Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2013). Understanding the role of the “self" in the social priming of mimicry. PLoS ONE, 8(4), e60249. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0060249
  • Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2014a). Why does gaze enhance mimicry? Placing gaze-mimicry effects in relation to other gaze phenomena. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(4), 747–762. doi:10.1080/17470218.2013.828316
  • Wang, Y., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2014b). Anterior medial prefrontal cortex implements social priming of mimicry. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. doi:10.1093/scan/nsu076
  • Wang, Y., Newport, R., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011). Eye contact enhances mimicry of intransitive hand movements. Biology Letters, 7(1), 7–10. doi:10.1098/rsbl.2010.0279
  • Wang, Y., Ramsey, R., & Hamilton, A. F. de C. (2011). The control of mimicry by eye contact is mediated by medial prefrontal cortex. The Journal of Neuroscience : The Official Journal of the Society for Neuroscience, 31(33), 12001–12010. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0845-11.2011
  • Wheeler, S. C., Demarree, K. G., & Petty, R. E. (2007). Understanding the role of the self in prime-to-behavior effects: The Active-Self account. Personality and Social Psychology Review : An Official Journal of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology, Inc, 11(3), 234–261. doi:10.1177/1088868307302223
  • Whiten, A., Horner, V., Litchfield, C. a., & Marshall-Pescini, S. (2004). How do apes ape? Animal Learning & Behavior, 32(1), 36–52. doi:10.3758/BF03196005
  • Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2417–2428. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0069
  • Wild, K. S., Poliakoff, E., Jerrison, A., & Gowen, E. (2012). Goal-directed and goal-less imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(8), 1739–1749. doi: 10.1007/s10803-011-1417-4
  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(3), 285–299. Retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15264497 doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000029551.56735.3a
  • Zental, T. R., & Galef, B. G. (1988). Social Learning: Psychological and Biological Perspectives (p. 368). Psychology Press. Retrieved from http://books.google.com/books?hl=en&lr=&id=mqFcAgAAQBAJ&pgis=1

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.