331
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Distributional analyses in the picture–word interference paradigm: Exploring the semantic interference and the distractor frequency effects

, &
Pages 1348-1369 | Received 26 Jan 2014, Accepted 08 Oct 2014, Published online: 06 Dec 2014

REFERENCES

  • Aarts, E., Roelofs, A., & van Turennout, M. (2009). Attentional control of task and response in lateral and medial frontal cortex: Brain activity and reaction time distributions. Neuropsychologia, 47, 2089–2099. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.019
  • Alario, F. X., & Ferrand, L. (1999). A set of 400 pictures standardized for French: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, and age of acquisition. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 31, 531–552. doi: 10.3758/BF03200732
  • Ayora, P., Peressotti, F., Alario, F.-X., Mulatti, C., Pluchino, P., Job, R., & Dell'Acqua, R. (2011). What phonological facilitation tells about semantic interference: A dual-task study. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 1–10. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00057
  • Balota, D. A., & Yap, M. J. (2011). Moving beyond the mean in studies of mental chronometry: The power of response time distributional analyses. Psychological Science, 20, 160–166.
  • Balota, D. A., Yap, M. J., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2008). Beyond mean response latency: Response time distributional analyses of semantic priming. Journal of Memory & Language, 59, 495–523. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.10.004
  • Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 34, 424–434. doi: 10.3758/BF03195471
  • Bonin, P., Peereman, R., Malardier, N., Mèot, A., & Chalard, M. (2003). A new set of 299 pictures for psycholinguistic studies: French norms for name agreement, image agreement, conceptual familiarity, visual complexity, image variability, age of acquisition, and naming latencies. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 35, 158–167. doi: 10.3758/BF03195507
  • Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 177–208. doi: 10.1080/026432997381664
  • Cousineau, D., Brown, S., & Heathcote, A. (2004). Fitting distributions using maximum likelihood: Methods and packages. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36, 742–756. doi: 10.3758/BF03206555
  • De Jong, R., Berendsen, E., & Cools, R. (1999). Goal neglect and inhibitory limitations: Dissociable causes of interference effects in conflict situations. Acta Psychologica, 101, 379–394. doi: 10.1016/S0001-6918(99)00012-8
  • Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychological Review, 93, 283–321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283
  • Dell'Acqua, R., Job, R., Peressotti, F., & Pascali, A. (2007). The picture-word interference effect is not a Stroop effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 717–722. doi: 10.3758/BF03196827
  • Dell'Acqua, R., Lotto, L., & Job, R. (2000). Naming times and standardized norms for the Italian PD/DPSS set of 266 pictures: Direct comparisons with American, English, French, and Spanish published databases. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32, 588–615. doi: 10.3758/BF03200832
  • Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2010). The distractor frequency effect in picture-word interference: Evidence for response exclusion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36, 878–891.
  • Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2011). The distractor frequency effect in a delayed picture-word interference task: Further evidence for a late locus of distractor exclusion. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 116–122. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0026-0
  • Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2012). Lexical selection and verbal self-monitoring: Effects of lexicality, context, and time pressure in picture-word interference. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 163–176. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.08.004
  • Dhooge, E., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2013). Distractor exclusion is not an early process: A reply to Roelofs, Piai, and Schriefers (2011). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 313–316.
  • Finkbeiner, M., & Caramazza, A. (2006). Now you see it, now you don't: On turning semantic interference into facilitation in a Stroop-like task. Cotrex, 42, 790–796.
  • Finocchiaro, C., & Navarrete, E. (2013). About the locus of the distractor frequency effect: Evidence from the production of clitic pronouns. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 861–872. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2013.832254
  • Forster, K. I., & Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: A Windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments & Computers, 35, 116–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03195503
  • Geng, J., Schnur, T. T., & Janssen, N. (2014). Relative speed of processing affects interference in Stroop and picture–word interference paradigms: Evidence from the distractor frequency effect. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29, 1100–1114. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2013.846473
  • Glaser, W. R., & Dungelhoff, F. J. (1984). The time course of picture–word interference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 640–654.
  • Heathcote, A., Brown, S., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (2002). Quantile maximum likelihood estimation of response time distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 394–401. doi: 10.3758/BF03196299
  • Heathcote, A., Popiel, S. J., & Mewhort, D. J. K. (1991). Analysis of response time distributions: An example using the Stroop task. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 340–347. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.340
  • Hommel, B. (1997). Interactions between stimulus-stimulus congruence and stimulus-response compatibility. Psychological Research, 59, 248–260. doi: 10.1007/BF00439302
  • Johnson, R. L., Staub, A., & Fleri, A. M. (2012). Distributional analysis of the transposed-letter neighborhood effect on naming latency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38, 1773–1779.
  • Kleinman, D. (2013). Resolving semantic interference during word production requires central attention. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 39, 1860–1877.
  • Laudanna, A., Thorton, A. M., Brown, G., Burani, C., & Marconi, L. (1995). Un corpus dell'italiano scritto contemporaneo dalla parte del ricevente. In S. Bolasco, L. Lebart, and A. Salem III (Eds.), Giornate Internazionali di Analisi Statistica dei Dati Testuali (Vol. I. pp. 103–109). Roma: Cisu.
  • Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 1–38.
  • Lupker, S. J. (1979). The semantic nature of response competition in the Picture–Word Interference Task. Memory and Cognition, 7, 485–495. doi: 10.3758/BF03198265
  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 163–203. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
  • Mahon, B. Z., Costa, A., Peterson, R., Vargas, K. A., & Caramazza, A. (2007). Lexical selection is not by competition: A reinterpretation of semantic interference and facilitation effects in the picture-word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 503–535.
  • Mahon, B. Z., Garcea, F. E., & Navarrete, E. (2012). Picture-word interference and the Response-exclusion hypothesis: A response to Mulatti and Coltheart. Cortex, 48, 373–377. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2011.10.008
  • Miozzo, M., & Caramazza, A. (2003). When more is less: A counterintuitive effect of distractor frequency in picture–word interference paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 228–252. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.2.228
  • Neely, J. H. (1991). Semantic priming effects in visual word recognition: A selective review of current findings and theories. In D. Besner & G. Humphreys (Eds.), Basic processes in reading: Visual word recognition (pp. 236–264). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Oomen, C. C. E., & Postma, A. (2002). Limitations in processing resources and speech monitoring. Language and Cognitive Processes, 17, 163–184. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000010
  • Pashler, H., & Johnston, J. C. (1989). Chronometric evidence for central postponement in temporally overlapping tasks. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 41, 19–45. doi: 10.1080/14640748908402351
  • Piai, V. & Roelofs, A. (2013). Working memory capacity and dual-task interference in picture naming. Acta Psychologica, 142, 332–342. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.01.006
  • Piai, V., Roelofs, A., & Schriefers, H. (2011). Semantic interference in immediate and delayed naming and reading: Attention and task decisions. Journal of Memory and Language, 64, 404–423. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2011.01.004
  • Piai, V., Roelofs, A., & Schriefers, H. (2012). Distractor strength and selective attention in picture-naming performance. Memory & Cognition, 40, 614–627. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0171-3
  • Piai, V., Roelofs, A., & Schriefers, H. (2014). Locus of semantic interference in picture naming: Evidence from dual-task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 147–165.
  • Postma, A. (2000). Detection of errors during speech production: A review of speech monitoring models. Cognition, 77, 97–132. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00090-1
  • Pratte, M. S., Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., & Feng, C. (2010). Exploring the differences in distributional properties between Stroop and Simon effects using delta plots. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 2013–2025. doi: 10.3758/APP.72.7.2013
  • Protopapas, A. (2007). CheckVocal: A program to facilitate checking the accuracy and response time of vocal responses from DMDX. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 859–862. doi: 10.3758/BF03192979
  • Ratcliff, R. (1979). Group reaction time distributions and an analysis of distribution statistics. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 446–461. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.3.446
  • Reynolds, M., & Besner, D. (2006). Reading aloud is not automatic: Processing capacity is required to generate a phonological code from print. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32, 1303–1323.
  • Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition, 42, 107–142. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-F
  • Roelofs, A. (1997). The WEAVER model of word-form encoding in speech production. Cognition, 64, 249–284. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(97)00027-9
  • Roelofs, A. (2003). Goal-referenced selection of verbal action: Modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 110, 88–125. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.110.1.88
  • Roelofs, A. (2005). From Popper to Lakatos: A case for cumulative computational modeling. In A. Cutler (Ed.), Twenty-first century psycholinguistics: Four cornerstones (pp. 313–330). Hillsdale, NJ: LEA.
  • Roelofs, A. (2008a). Dynamics of the attentional control of word retrieval: Analyses of response time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137, 303–323. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.137.2.303
  • Roelofs, A. (2008b). Tracing attention and the activation flow in spoken word planning using eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 353–368.
  • Roelofs, A. (2012). Attention, spatial integration, and the tail of response time distributions in Stroop task performance. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 135–150. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2011.605152
  • Roelofs, A., Piai, V., & Garrido Rodriguez, G. (2011a). Attentional inhibition in bilingual naming performance: Evidence from delta-plot analyses. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, article 184.
  • Roelofs, A., Piai, V., & Schriefers, H. (2011b). Selective attention and distractor frequency in naming performance: Comment on Dhooge and Hartsuiker (2010). Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 37, 1032–1038.
  • Rosinski, R. R. (1977). Picture–word interference is semantically based. Child Development, 48, 643–647. doi: 10.2307/1128667
  • Rouder, J. N., Lu, J., Speckman, P., Sun, D., & Jiang, Y. (2005). A hierarchical model for estimating response time distributions. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 195–223. doi: 10.3758/BF03257252
  • Scaltritti, M., Balota, D. A., & Peressotti, F. (2013). Exploring the additive effects of stimulus quality and word frequency: The influence of local and list-wide prime relatedness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 66, 91–107. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.698628
  • Schnur, T. T., & Martin, R. (2012). Semantic picture-word interference is a post-perceptual effect. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 19, 301–308. doi: 10.3758/s13423-011-0190-x
  • Shao, Z., Meyer, A. S., & Roelofs, A. (2013). Selective and nonselective inhibition of competitors in picture naming. Memory & Cognition, 41, 1200–1211. doi: 10.3758/s13421-013-0332-7
  • Shao, Z., Roelofs, A., & Meyer, A. S. (2012). Sources of individual differences in the speed of naming objects and actions: The contribution of executive control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 1927–1944. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2012.670252
  • Spieler, H. D., Balota, D. A., & Faust, M. E. (2000). Levels of selective attention revealed through analyses of response time distributions. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Attention and Performance, 26, 506–526.
  • Steinhouser, M., & Hübner, R. (2009). Distinguishing response conflict and task conflict in the Stroop task: Evidence from ex-Gaussian distribution analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 35, 1398–1412.
  • Thomas, M. A., Neely, J. H., & O'Connor, P. (2012). When word identification gets tough, retrospective semantic processing comes to rescue. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 623–643. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.02.002
  • van Maanen, L., van Rijn, H., & Taatgen, N. (2012). RACE/A: An architectural account of the interactions between learning, task control, and retrieval dynamics. Cognitive Science, 36, 62–101. doi: 10.1111/j.1551-6709.2011.01213.x
  • Van Selst, M., & Jolicoeur, P. (1994). A solution to the effect of sample size on outlier elimination. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 47, 631–650. doi: 10.1080/14640749408401131
  • White, S. J., & Staub, A. (2012). The distribution of fixation durations during reading: Effects of stimulus quality. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 603–617.
  • Yap, M. J., Tse, C. S., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Individual differences in the joint effects of semantic priming and word frequency revealed by RT distributional analyses: The role of lexical integrity. Journal of Memory and Language, 61, 303–325. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2009.07.001

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.