197
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Regular articles

Developmental changes in misinterpretation of garden-path wh-questions in French

, &
Pages 829-854 | Received 22 Apr 2014, Accepted 12 May 2015, Published online: 15 Jun 2015

REFERENCES

  • Aoshima, S., Phillips, C., & Weinberg, A. (2004). Processing filler-gap dependencies in a head-final language. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 23–54. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.03.001
  • Baayen, R. H., Davidson, D. J., & Bates, D. M. (2008). Mixed-effects modeling with crossed random effects for subjects and items. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 390–412. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.12.005
  • Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255–278. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001
  • Cheng, L. L-S., & Rooryck, J. (2000). Licensing Wh-in-situ. Syntax, 3(1), 1–19. doi: 10.1111/1467-9612.00022
  • Choi, Y., & Trueswell, J. C. (2010). Children's (in)ability to recover from garden paths in a verb-final language: Evidence for developing control in sentence processing. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 106, 41–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.01.003
  • Christianson, K., Hollingworth, A., Halliwell, J. F., & Ferreira, F. (2001). Thematic roles assigned along the garden path linger. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 368–407. doi: 10.1006/cogp.2001.0752
  • Crain, S., & Fodor, J. D. (1985). How can grammars help parsers. In D. R. Dowty, L. Karttunen, & A. M. Zwicky (Eds.), Natural language parsing: Psychological, computational, and theoretical perspectives (pp. 94–128). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • De Villiers, J., Roeper, T., Bland-Stewart, L., & Pearson, B. (2008). Answering hard questions: Wh-movement across dialects and disorder. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 67–103. doi: 10.1017/S0142716408080041
  • De Villiers, J., Roeper, T., & Vainikka, A. (1990). The acquisition of long-distance rules. In L. Frazier & J. De Villiers (Eds.), Language processing and language acquisition (pp. 257–297). Dordrecht: Kluwer.
  • Drummond, A. (2013). Ibex farm. Retrieved from http://spellout.net/ibexfarm/
  • Ferreira, F. (2003). The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences. Cognitive Psychology, 47(2), 164–203. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0285(03)00005-7
  • Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1991). Recovery from misanalyses of garden-path sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 30, 725–745. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90034-H
  • Ferreira, F., & Henderson, J. M. (1998). Syntactic reanalysis, thematic processing, and sentence comprehension. In J. D. Fodor & F. Ferreira (Eds.), Studies in Theoretical Psycholinguistics, Volume 21 (pp. 73–100). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • Ferreira, F., & Patson, N. D. (2007). The ‘good enough’ approach to language comprehension. Language and Linguistics Compass, 1, 71–83. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2007.00007.x
  • Fiebach, C. J., Schlesewsky, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2002). Separating syntactic memory costs and syntactic integration costs during parsing: The processing of German WH-questions. Journal of Memory and Language, 47, 250–272. doi: 10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00004-9
  • Fine, A. B., Jaeger, T. F., Farmer, T. A., & Qian, T. (2013). Rapid expectation adaptation during syntactic comprehension. PLoS ONE, 8(10), 1–18. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0077661
  • Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (1994). The diagnosis and cure of garden paths. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23(5), 407–434. doi: 10.1007/BF02143947
  • Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (1998). Attach anyway. In J. D. Fodor & Ferreira (Eds.), Reanalysis in sentence processing (pp. 101–141). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.
  • Fodor, J. D., & Inoue, A. (2000). Syntactic features in reanalysis: Positive and negative symptoms. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(1), 25–36. doi: 10.1023/A:1005168206061
  • Frazier, L., & Clifton, C. Jr. (1998). Sentence reanalysis, and visibility. In J. D. Fodor & F. Ferreira (Eds.), Reanalysis in sentence processing (pp. 143–176). Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Frazier, L. & Flores d'Arcais, G. B. F. (1989). Filler driven parsing: A study of gap filling in dutch. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 331–344. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(89)90037-5
  • Garnsey, S. M., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Chapman, R. M. (1989). Evoked potentials and the study of sentence comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 18, 51–60. doi: 10.1007/BF01069046
  • Gerken, L. (2006). Decisions, decisions: Infant language learning when multiple generalizations are possible. Cognition, 98(3), B67–B74. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.03.003
  • Gibson, E. (1998). Linguistic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies. Cognition, 68(1), 1–76. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00034-1
  • Gomez, R. L. (2002). Variability and detection of invariant structure. Psychological Science, 13, 431–436. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00476
  • Gordon, P. C., Hendrick, R., & Levine, W. H. (2002). Memory-load interference in syntactic processing. Psychological Science, 13(5), 425–430. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00475
  • Hale, J. (2003). The information conveyed by words in sentences. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32(2), 101–123. doi: 10.1023/A:1022492123056
  • Huang, Y. T., Zheng, X., Meng, X., & Snedeker, J. (2013). Children's assignment of grammatical roles in the online processing of Mandarin passive sentences. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(4), 589–606. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.08.002
  • Hurewitz, F., Brown-Schmidt, S., Thorpe, K., Gleitman, L. R., & Trueswell, J. C. (2000). One frog, two frog, red frog, blue frog: Factors affecting children's syntactic choices in production and comprehension. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29, 597–626. doi: 10.1023/A:1026468209238
  • Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59(4), 434–446. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007
  • Kaschak, M. P., & Glenberg, A. M. (2004). This construction needs learned. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 133(3), 450–467. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.450
  • King, J., & Just, M. A. (1991). Individual differences in syntactic processing: The role of working memory* 1. Journal of Memory and Language, 30(5), 580–602. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(91)90027-H
  • Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., & Christensen, R. H. B. (2014). lmerTest: Tests for random and fixed effects for linearmixed effect models. R Package Version 2.0–11. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest.
  • Lassotta, R., Omak, A., Panizza, D., Villata, S., & Franck, J. (2015). Linking cognitive control and syntactic revision of garden-path wh-questions in adults and children. Manuscript in preparation.
  • Levin, B., & Rappaport-Hovav, M. R. (1995). Unaccusativity: At the syntax-lexical semantics interface. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.
  • Levy, R. (2008). A noisy-channel model of rational human sentence comprehension under uncertain input. Proceedings of the 13th Conference on Empirical Methods in Natural Language Processing (Association for Computational Linguistics, Stroudsburg, PA), 234–243.
  • Lewis, R. L. (1998). Reanalysis and limited repair parsing: Leaping off the garden path. In F. Ferreira & J. D. Fodor (Eds.), Reanalysis in sentence processing (pp. 247–286). Dordrecht: Springer.
  • Love, T. E. (2007). The processing of non-canonically ordered constituents in long distance dependencies by pre-school children: A real-time investigation. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 191–206. doi: 10.1007/s10936-006-9040-9
  • Marslen-Wilson, W. (1975). Sentence perception as an interactive parallel process. Science, 189, 226–228. doi: 10.1126/science.189.4198.226
  • Mathieu, E. (2004). The mapping of form and interpretation: The case of optional wh-movement in french. Lingua, 114(9–10), 1090–1132. doi: 10.1016/j.lingua.2003.07.002
  • Maye, J., Werker, J. F., & Gerken, L. (2002). Infant sensitivity to distributional information can affect phonetic discrimination. Cognition, 82, B101–B111. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00157-3
  • Mazuka, R., Jincho, N., & Oishi, H. (2009). Development of executive control and language processing. Language and Linguistics Compass, 3, 59–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2008.00102.x
  • Meroni, L., & Crain, S. (2003). On not being led down the kindergarten path. In B. Beachley, A. Brown, & F. Conlin (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27th boston university conference on language development (pp. 531–544). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.
  • Myers, L. L. (2007). WH-interrogatives in spoken French: A corpus-based analysis of their form and function. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Texas at Austin.
  • Novick, J. M., Hussey, E., Teubner-Rhodes, S., Harbison, J. I., & Bunting, M. F. (2013). Clearing the garden-path: Improving sentence processing through cognitive control training. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 29(2), 186–217. doi: 10.1080/01690965.2012.758297
  • Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2005). Cognitive control and parsing: Reexamining the role of broca's area in sentence comprehension. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 5, 263–281. doi: 10.3758/CABN.5.3.263
  • Novick, J. M., Trueswell, J. C., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2010). Broca's area and language processing: Evidence for the cognitive control connection. Language and Linguistics Compass, 4, 906–924. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-818X.2010.00244.x
  • Oiry, M. (2010). A case of true optionality: Wh-in-situ patterns like long movement in French. In A. Roussou & C. Vlachos (Eds.), The optionality of wh-Movement. Linguistic Analysis, 37(1–2), 112–136.
  • Omaki, A., Davidson White, I., Goro, T., Lidz, J., & Phillips, C. (2014). No fear of commitment: Children's incremental interpretation in English and Japanese wh questions. Language Learning and Development, 0, 1–28.
  • Omaki, A., & Lidz, J. (2015). Linking parser development to acquisition of syntactic knowledge. Language Acquisition, 22, 158–192. doi: 10.1080/10489223.2014.943903
  • Omaki, A., & Schulz, B. (2011). Filler-gap dependencies and island constraints in second-language sentence processing. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 33, 563–588. doi: 10.1017/S0272263111000313
  • Perlmutter, D. (1978). Impersonal passives and the unaccusative hypothesis. Proceedings of the fourth annual meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, Berkeley, pp. 157–189.
  • Phillips, C., & Wagers, M. (2007). Relating structure and time in linguistics and psycholinguistics. In G. Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 739–756). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • R Development Core Team. (2013). R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R foundation for Statistical Computing. Retrieved from http://www.R-project.org/
  • Roberts, L., Marinis, T., Felser, C., & Clahsen, H. (2007). Antecedent priming at trace positions in children's sentence processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 175–188. doi: 10.1007/s10936-006-9038-3
  • Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, New Series, 274, 1926–1928.
  • Schütze, C. T., & Sprouse, J. (2013). Judgment data. In R. J. Podesva & D. Sharma (Eds.), Research methods in linguistics (pp. 27–50). Cambridge: Cambride University Press.
  • Slattery, T. J., Sturt, P., Christianson, K., Yoshida, M., & Ferreira, F. (2013). Lingering misinterpretations of garden path sentences arise from competing syntactic representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(2), 104–120. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.04.001
  • Snedeker, J. (2013). Children's sentence processing. In R. P. G. van Gompel (Ed.), Sentence processing (pp. 189–220). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Staub, A. (2007). The return of the repressed: Abandoned parses facilitate syntactic reanalysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(2), 299–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.09.001
  • Stowe, A. (1986). Parsing wh-constructions: Evidence for on-line gap location. Language and Cognitive Processes, 1, 227–245. doi: 10.1080/01690968608407062
  • Sturt, P. (2007). Semantic re-interpretation and garden path recovery. Cognition, 105, 477–488. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2006.10.009
  • Sussman, R. S., & Sedivy, J. (2003). The time-course of processing syntactic dependencies: Evidence from eye movements. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18(2), 143–163. doi: 10.1080/01690960143000498
  • Tabor, W., Galantucci, B., & Richardson, D. (2004). Effects of merely local syntactic coherence on sentence processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(4), 355–370. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.001
  • Tanenhaus, K., Spivey-Knowlton, J., Eberhard, M., & Sedivy, C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic information in spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 1632–1634. doi: 10.1126/science.7777863
  • Thompson, S. P., & Newport, E. L. (2007). Statistical learning of syntax: The role of transitional probability. Language Learning and Development, 3(1), 1–42. doi: 10.1080/15475440709336999
  • Traxler, M. J., & Pickering, M. J. (1996). Plausibility and the processing of unbounded dependencies: An eye-tracking study. Journal of Memory and Language, 35, 454–475. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1996.0025
  • Trueswell, J. C., & Gleitman, L. R. (2007). Learning to parse and its implications for language acquisition. In Gaskell (Ed.), Oxford handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 635–656). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in young children. Cognition, 73, 89–134. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(99)00032-3
  • Unsworth, S. (2013). Assessing the role of current and cumulative exposure in simultaneous bilingual acquisition: The case of Dutch gender. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 16, 86–110. doi: 10.1017/S1366728912000284
  • Van Dyke, J. A., & McElree, B. (2006). Retrieval interference in sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(2), 157–166. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.007
  • van Gompel, R. P. G., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., & Jacob, G. (2006). The activation of inappropriate analyses in garden-path sentences: Evidence from structural priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 55(3), 335–362. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2006.06.004
  • Wagers, M. W., & Phillips, C. (2014). Going the distance: Memory and control processes in active dependency construction. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 67(7), 1274–1304. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.858363
  • Weighall, A. R. (2008). The kindergarten path effect revisited: Children's use of context in processing structural ambiguities. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 99, 75–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2007.10.004
  • Xiang, M., Dillon, B., Wagers, M., Liu, F. Q., & Guo, T. M. (2014). Processing covert dependencies:An SAT study on Mandarin wh-in-situ questions. Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 23, 207–232. doi: 10.1007/s10831-013-9115-1

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.