500
Views
46
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
EPS Prize Lecture

Distorted body representations in healthy cognition

Pages 378-388 | Received 12 Aug 2015, Accepted 23 Nov 2015, Published online: 31 Mar 2016

References

  • Alloway, K. D., Rosenthal, P., & Burton, H. (1989). Quantitative measurements of receptive field changes during antagonism of GABAergic transmission in primary somatosensory cortex of cats. Experimental Brain Research, 78, 514–532. doi: 10.1007/BF00230239
  • Anema, H. A., Wolswijk, V. W., Ruis, C., & Dijkerman, H. C. (2008). Grasping Weber's illusion: The effect of receptor density differences on grasping and matching. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 951–967. doi: 10.1080/02643290802041323
  • Bermúdez, J. L., Marcel, A., & Eilan, N. (1995). The body and the self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Berti, A., Bottini, G., Gandola, M., Pia, L., Smania, N., Stracciari, A., … Paulesu, E. (2005). Shared cortical anatomy for motor awareness and motor control. Science, 309, 488–491. doi:10.1126/science.1110625
  • Blanke, O., Landis, T., Spinelli, L., & Seeck, M. (2004). Out-of-body experience and autoscopy of neurological origin. Brain, 127, 243–258. doi: 10.1093/brain/awh040
  • Blanke, O., & Metzinger, T. (2009). Full-body illusions and minimal phenomenal selfhood. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13, 7–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.10.003
  • Bookstein, F. L. (1991). Morphometric tools for landmark data: Geometry and biology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hand “feel” touch that eyes seen. Nature, 391, 756. doi: 10.1038/35784
  • Brooks, V. B., Rudomin, P., & Slayman, C. L. (1961). Peripheral receptive fields of neurons in the cat's cerebral cortex. Journal of Neurophysiology, 24, 302–325.
  • Bruch, H. (1978). The golden cage: The enigma of anorexia nervosa. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Brugger, P., Kollias, S. S., Müri, R. M., Crelier, G., Hepp-Reymond, M.-C., & Regard, M. (2000). Beyond re-membering: Phantom sensations of congenitally absent limbs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 97, 6167–6172. doi: 10.1073/pnas.100510697
  • Brugger, P., Lenggenhager, B., & Giummarra, M. J. (2013). Xenomelia: A social neuroscience view of altered bodily self-consciousness. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 204. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00204
  • Brugger, P., Regard, M., & Landis, T. (1997). Illusory reduplication of one's own body: Phenomenology and classification of autoscopic phenomena. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 2, 19–38. doi: 10.1080/135468097396397
  • Canzoneri, E., Ubaldi, S., Rastelli, V., Finisguerra, A., Bassolino, M., & Serino, A. (2013). Tool-use reshapes the boundaries of body and peripersonal space representations. Experimental Brain Research, 228, 25–42. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3532-2
  • Cardini, F., Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2011). Vision of the body modulates somatosensory intracortical inhibition. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 2014–2022. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhq267
  • Casper, R. C., Halmi, K. A., Goldberg, S. C., Eckert, E. D., & Davis, J. M. (1979). Disturbances in body image estimation as related to other characteristics and outcome in anorexia nervosa. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 60–66.
  • Cholewiak, R. W. (1999). The perception of tactile distance: Influences of body site, space, and time. Perception, 28, 851–875. doi: 10.1068/p2873
  • Craske, B., Kenny, F. T., & Keith, D. (1984). Modifying an underlying component of perceived arm length: Adaptation of tactile location induced by spatial discordance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 10, 307–317.
  • Critchley, M. (1953). The parietal lobes. London: Edward Arnold & Co.
  • Daurat-Hmeljiak, C., Stambak, M., & Berges, J. (1978). Il test dello schema corporeo. Una prova di conoscenza e costruzione dell'immagine del corpo [The body schema test: An examination of the knowledge and construction of the body image]. Florence, Italy: Organizzazioni Speciali.
  • de Vignemont, F., Ehrsson, H. H., & Haggard, P. (2005). Bodily illusions modulate tactile perception. Current Biology, 15, 1286–1290. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.067
  • de Vignemont, F., Majid, A., Jola, C., & Haggard, P. (2009). Segmenting the body into parts: Evidence from biases in tactile perception. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62, 500–512. doi: 10.1080/17470210802000802
  • DiCarlo, J. J., & Johnson, K. O. (2002). Receptive field structure in cortical area 3b of the alert monkey. Behavioural Brain Research, 135, 167–178. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(02)00162-6
  • DiCarlo, J. J., Johnson, K. O., & Hsiao, S. S. (1998). Structure of receptive fields in area 3b of primary somatosensory cortex in the alert monkey. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 2626–2645.
  • Duncan, R. O., & Boynton, G. M. (2007). Tactile hyperacuity thresholds correlate with finger maps in primary somatosensory cortex (S1). Cerebral Cortex, 17, 2878–2891. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm015
  • Fairburn, C. G., Peveler, R. C., Jones, R., Hope, R. A., & Doll, H. A. (1993). Predictors of 12-month outcome in bulimia nervosa and the influence of attitudes to shape and weight. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 61, 696–698. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.61.4.696
  • Ferrè, E., Vagnoni, E., & Haggard, P. (2013). Vestibular contributions to bodily awareness. Neuropsychologia, 51, 1445–1452. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.04.006
  • First, M. B. (2005). Desire for amputation of a limb: Paraphilia, psychosis, or a new type of identity disorder. Psychological Medicine, 35, 919–928. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704003320
  • Fotopoulou, A., Tsakiris, M., Haggard, P., Vagopoulou, A., Rudd, A., & Kopelman, M. (2008). The role of motor intention in motor awareness: An experimental study on anosognosia for hemiplegia. Brain, 131, 3432–3442. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn225
  • Fuentes, C. T., Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2013). Body image distortions in healthy adults. Acta Psychologica, 144, 344–351. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.06.012
  • Fuentes, C. T., Pazzaglia, M., Longo, M. R., Scivoletto, G., & Haggard, P. (2013). Body image distortions following spinal cord injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 84, 201–207. doi: 10.1136/jnnp-2012-304001
  • Fuentes, C. T., Runa, C., Blanco, X. A., Orvalho, V., & Haggard, P. (2013). Does my face FIT? A face image task reveals structure and distortions of facial feature representation. PLOS ONE, 8, e76805. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076805
  • Gandevia, S. C., & Phegan, C. M. (1999). Perceptual distortions of the human body image produced by local anesthesia, pain and cutaneous stimulation. Journal of Physiology, 514, 609–616. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1999.609ae.x
  • Green, B. E. (1982). The perception of distance and location for dual tactile pressures. Perception and Psychophysics, 31, 315–323. doi: 10.3758/BF03202654
  • Gurfinkel, V. S., & Levick, Y. S. (1991). Perceptual and automatic aspects of the postural body scheme. In J. Paillard (Ed.), Brain and space (pp. 147–162). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.
  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York: Dover.
  • Keel, P. K., Dorer, D. J., Franko, D. L., Jackson, S. C., & Herzog, D. B. (2005). Postremission predictors of relapse in women with eating disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 2263–2268. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2263
  • Kennett, S., Taylor-Clarke, M., & Haggard, P. (2001). Noninformative vision improves the spatial resolution of touch in humans. Current Biology, 11, 1188–1191. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(01)00327-X
  • Le Cornu Knight, F., Longo, M. R., & Bremner, A. J. (2014). Categorical perception of tactile distance. Cognition, 131, 254–262. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2014.01.005
  • Lenggenhager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., & Blanke, O. (2007). Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science, 317, 1096–1099. doi: 10.1126/science.1143439
  • Linkenauger, S. A., Witt, J. K., Bakdash, J. Z., Stefanucci, J. K., & Proffitt, D. R. (2009). Asymmetrical body perception: A possible role for neural body representations. Psychological Science, 20, 1373–1380. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02447.x
  • Linkenauger, S. A., Wong, H. Y., Guess, M., Stefanucci, J. K., McCulloch, K. C., Bülthoff, H. H., … Proffitt, D. R. (2015). The perceptual homunculus: The perception of the relative proportions of the human body. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144, 103–113. doi:10.1037/xge0000028
  • Longo, M. R. (2014). The effects of immediate vision on implicit hand maps. Experimental Brain Research, 232, 1241–1247. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3840-1
  • Longo, M. R. (2015a). Implicit and explicit body representations. European Psychologist, 20, 6–15. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000198
  • Longo, M. R. (2015b). Posture modulates implicit hand maps. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 96–102. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.009
  • Longo, M. R. (2015c). Three-dimensional coherence of the conscious body image. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68, 1116–1123. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.975731
  • Longo, M. R. (2015d). Intuitive anatomy: Distorted conceptual knowledge of hand structure. Cognition, 142, 230–235. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.024
  • Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2010). An implicit body representation underlying human position sense. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107, 11727–11732. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1003483107
  • Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2011). Weber's illusion and body shape: Anisotropy of tactile size perception on the hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 37, 720–726.
  • Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2012a). A 2.5-D representation of the human hand. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 38, 9–13.
  • Longo, M. R., & Haggard, P. (2012b). Implicit body representations and the conscious body image. Acta Psychologica, 141, 164–168. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2012.07.015
  • Longo, M. R., & Sadibolova, R. (2013). Seeing the body distorts tactile size perception. Cognition, 126, 475–481. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.11.013
  • Longo, M. R., Azañón, E., & Haggard, P. (2010). More than skin deep: Body representation beyond primary somatosensory cortex. Neuropsychologia, 48, 655–668. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.022
  • Longo, M. R., Betti, V., Aglioti, S. M., & Haggard, P. (2009). Visually induced analgesia: Seeing the body reduces pain. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 12125–12130. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3072-09.2009
  • Longo, M. R., Ghosh, A., & Yahya, T. (2015). Bilateral symmetry of distortions of tactile size perception. Perception, 44, 1251–1262.
  • Longo, M. R., Long, C., & Haggard, P. (2012). Mapping the invisible hand: A body model of a phantom limb. Psychological Science, 23, 740–742. doi: 10.1177/0956797612441219
  • Longo, M. R., Mancini, F., & Haggard, P. (2015). Implicit body representations and tactile spatial remapping. Acta Psychologica, 160, 77–87. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.07.002
  • Longo, M. R., Pernigo, S., & Haggard, P. (2011). Vision of the body modulates processing in primary somatosensory cortex. Neuroscience Letters, 489, 159–163. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.12.007
  • Longo, M. R., Schüür, F., Kammers, M. P. M., Tsakiris, M., & Haggard, P. (2008). What is embodiment? A psychometric approach. Cognition, 107, 978–998. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.12.004
  • Mancini, F., Longo, M. R., Iannetti, G. D., & Haggard, P. (2011). A supramodal representation of the body surface. Neuropsychologia, 49, 1194–1201. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.12.040
  • Mancini, F., Longo, M. R., Kammers, M. P. M., & Haggard, P. (2011). Visual distortion of body size modulates pain perception. Psychological Science, 22, 325–330. doi: 10.1177/0956797611398496
  • Margolis, A. N., & Longo, M. R. (2015). Visual detail about the body modulates tactile localisation biases. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 351–358. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-4118-3
  • Mattioni, S., & Longo, M. R. (2014). The effects of verbal cueing on implicit hand maps. Acta Psychologica, 153, 60–65. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2014.09.009
  • Melzack, R. (1990). Phantom limbs and the concept of a neuromatrix. Trends in Neuroscience, 13, 88–92. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(90)90179-E
  • Melzack, R. (1992). Phantom limbs. Scientific American, 266(4), 120–125. doi:10.1038/scientificamerican0492-120
  • Miller, L. E., Longo, M. R., & Saygin, A. P. (2014). Tool morphology constrains the effects of tool use on body representations. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 2143–2153.
  • Penfield, W., & Boldrey, E. (1937). Somatic motor and sensory representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain, 60, 389–443. doi: 10.1093/brain/60.4.389
  • Phillips, K. A., Didie, E. R., Feusner, J., & Wilhelm, S. (2008). Body dysmorphic disorder: Treating an underrecognized disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1111–1118. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08040500
  • Powell, T. P. S., & Mountcastle, V. B. (1959). Some aspects of the functional organization of the cortex of the postcentral gyrus of the monkey: A correlation of findings obtained in a single unit analysis with cytoarchitecture. Bulletin of the Johns Hopkins Hospital, 105, 133–162.
  • Proske, U., & Gandevia, S. C. (2012). The proprioceptive senses: Their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiological Reviews, 92, 1651–1697. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011
  • Ramachandran, V. S., & Hirstein, W. (1998). The perception of phantom limbs: The D. O. Hebb Lecture. Brain, 121, 1603–1630. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.9.1603
  • Sadibolova, R., & Longo, M. R. (2014). Seeing the body produces limb-specific modulation of skin temperature. Biology Letters, 10, 20140157. doi: 10.1098/rsbl.2014.0157
  • Saulton, A., Dodds, T. J., Bülthoff, H. H., & de la Rosa, S. (2015). Objects exhibit body model like shape distortions. Experimental Brain Research, 233, 1471–1479. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4221-0
  • Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2009). Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3, 214–220. doi: 10.3389/neuro.01.029.2009
  • Soechting, J. F. (1982). Does position sense at the elbow reflect a sense of elbow joint angle of one of limb orientation? Brain Research, 248, 392–395. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90601-1
  • Sur, M., Merzenich, M. M., & Kaas, J. H. (1980). Magnification, receptive-field area, and ‘‘hypercolumn’’ size in areas 3b and 1 of somatosensory cortex in owl monkeys. Journal of Neurophysiology, 44, 295–311.
  • Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Väljamäe, A., Toshima, I., Kimura, T., Tsakiris, M., & Kitagawa, N. (2012). Action sounds recalibrate perceived tactile distance. Current Biology, 22, R516–R517. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.04.028
  • Taylor-Clarke, M., Jacobsen, P., & Haggard, P. (2004). Keeping the world a constant size: Object constancy in human touch. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 219–220. doi: 10.1038/nn1199
  • Treasure, J., Claudino, A. M., & Zucker, N. (2010). Eating disorders. Lancet, 375, 583–593. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61748-7
  • Türker, K. S., Yeo, P. L., & Gandevia, S. C. (2005). Perceptual distortion of face deletion by local anaesthesia of the human lips and teeth. Experimental Brain Research, 165, 37–43. doi: 10.1007/s00221-005-2278-x
  • Vallar, G., & Ronchi, R. (2009). Somatoparaphrenia: A body delusion. A review of the neuropsychological literature. Experimental Brain Research, 192, 533–551.
  • van Beers, R. J., Sittig, A. C., & Denier van der Gon, J. J. (1998). The precision of proprioceptive position sense. Experimental Brain Research, 122, 367–377. doi: 10.1007/s002210050525
  • Vega-Bermudez, F., & Johnson, K. O. (2001). Differences in spatial acuity between digits. Neurology, 56, 1389–1391. doi: 10.1212/WNL.56.10.1389
  • Weber, E. H. (1996). De subtilitate tactus ( H. E. Ross, Trans.). In H. E. Ross & D. J. Murray (Eds.), E. H. Weber on the tactile senses (2nd ed., pp. 21–128). Hove, UK: Erlbaum. (Original work published in 1834).

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.