325
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Processing Semitic Scripts: Reading and Writing in Arabic and Hebrew

The effect of the internal orthographic connectivity of written Arabic words on the process of the visual recognition: A comparison between skilled and dyslexic readers

, , &
Pages 214-233 | Published online: 08 Oct 2013

REFERENCES

  • Abdelhadi, S., Ibrahim, R., & Eviatar, Z. (2011). Perceptual load in the reading of Arabic: Effects of orthographic visual complexity on detection. Writing Systems Research, 3, 117–127. doi:10.1093/wsr/wsr014
  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1995). Learning to read in Arabic: Reading, syntactic, orthographic and working memory skills in normally achieving and poor Arabic readers. Reading Psychology, 16(4), 351–394. doi:10.1080/0270271950160401
  • Abu-Rabia, S. Awwad. J. (2004). Morphological structures in visual word recognition: The case of Arabic. Journal of Research in Reading, 27(3), 321–336. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.2004.00235.x
  • Abu-Rabia, S., Share, D., & Mansour, M. S. (2003). Word recognition and basic cognitive processes among reading-disabled and normal readers in Arabic. Reading and Writing, 16(5), 423–442. doi:10.1023/A:102423741514
  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2004). Reading and spelling error analysis of native. Reading and Writing, 17(7–8), 651–690. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2657-x
  • Abu-Rabia, S., & Taha, H. (2006). Phonological errors predominate in Arabic spelling across grades 1–9. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35(2), 167–188. doi:10.1007/s10936-005-9010-7
  • Alvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., & de Vega, M. (2000). Syllable-frequency effect in visual word recognition: Evidence of a sequential-type processing. Psicolo′gica, 21, 341–374. http://www.uv.es/revispsi/articulos3.00/mono/monoL3.pdf
  • Bar-Kochva, I. (2011). Does processing a shallow and a deep orthography produce different brain activity patterns? An ERP study conducted in Hebrew. Developmental Neuropsychology, 36(7), 933–938. doi:10.1080/87565641.2011.606417
  • Bentin, S., Hammer, R., & Cahan, S. (1991). The effects of aging and first-grade schooling on the development of phonological awareness. Psychological Science, 2(4), 271–274. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1991.tb00148.x
  • Bentin, S., McCarthy, G., & Wood, C. C. (1985). Event-related potentials, lexical decision and semantic priming. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 60, 343–355. doi:10.1016/0013-4694(85)90008-2
  • Bentin, S., Mouchetant-Rostaing, Y., Giard, M. H., Echallier, J. F., & Pernier, J. (1999). ERP manifestations of processing printed words at different psycholinguistic levels: time course and scalp distribution. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(3), 235–260. doi:10.1093/cercor/6.1.21
  • Breznitz, Z. (2004). Introduction on regular and impaired reading in Semitic languages. Reading and writing, 17, 645–649. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2652-2
  • Breznitz, Z., & Misra, M. (2003). Speed of processing of the visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological systems in adult dyslexics: The contribution of “asynchrony” to word recognition deficits. Brain and Language, 85(3), 486–502. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00071-3
  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 439–45. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.439
  • Brysbaert, M., & Praet, C. (1992). Reading isolated words: No evidence for automatic incorporation of the phonetic code. Psychological Research, 54(2), 91–10. doi:10.1007/BF00937137
  • Bybee, J. (1995). Diachronic and typological properties of morphology and their implication for representation. In L. B. Feldman (Eds.), Morphological aspects of language processing (pp. 225–246). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
  • Castles, A., & Coltheart, M. (1993). Varieties of developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 47(2), 149–180. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90003-E
  • Cattell, J. (1886). The time taken up by cerebral operations. Mind, 11, 277–282, 524–538. doi:10.1093/mind/os-XI.42.220
  • Coltheart, M. (2005). Modelling reading: The dual-route approach. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading (pp. 6–23). Oxford: Blackwells Publishing.
  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.1.204
  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82(4), 733–740. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.733
  • Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidence from eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42(4), 317–367. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0750
  • Davis, C. J., & Lupker, S. J. (2006). Masked inhibitory priming in English: Evidence for lexical inhibition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(3), 668–687. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.3.668
  • Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Sigman, M., & Vinckier, F. (2005). The neural code for written words: A proposal. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(7), 335–341. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2005.05.004
  • Denckla, M. B., & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid “automatized” naming (R.A.N): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities. Neuropsychologia, 14(4), 471–479. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(76)90075-0
  • Ehri, L. C. (1987). Learning to read and spell words. Journal of Literacy Research, 19(1) 5–31. doi:10.1080/10862968709547585
  • Ehri, L. C. (2000). Learning to read and learning to spell: Two sides of a coin. Topics in Language Disorders, 20(3), 19–36. doi:10.1097/00011363-200020030-00005
  • Ehri, L. C., & Saltmarsh, J. (1995). Beginning readers outperform older disabled readers in learning to read words by sight. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7(3), 295–326. doi:10.1007/BF03162082
  • Ehri, C. L., & Snowling, J. M. (2005). Developmental variations in word recognition. In C. A. Stone, R. E. Silliman, J. B. Ehren & K. Apel (Eds.), Handbook of language and literacy: Development and disorders (pp. 433–460). New York, NY: Guilford Press.
  • Elbeheri, G., & Everatt, J. (2007). Literacy ability and phonological processing skills amongst disabled readers and non-disabled readers speakers of Arabic. Reading &Writing, 20, 273–294. doi:10.1007/s11145-006-9031-0
  • E-prime (2.0) [Computer software]. http://www.pstnet.com/
  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2000). Bilingual is as bilingual does: Metalinguistic abilities of Arabic-speaking children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21(4), 451–471. doi:10.1017/S0142716400004021
  • Eviatar, Z., & Ibrahim, R. (2004). Morphological and orthographic effects on hemispheric processing of nonwords. A cross-linguistic comparison. Reading and Writing, 17(7–8), 691–705. doi:10.1007/s11145-004-2659-8
  • Eviatar, Z., Ibrahim, R., & Ganayim, D. (2004). Orthography and the hemispheres: visual and linguistic aspects of letter processing. Neuropsychology., 18(1), 174–184. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.174
  • Ferguson, C. A. (1959). Diglossia. Word, 14, 47–56
  • Forster, K. I., & Shen, D. (1996). No enemies in the neighborhood: Absence of inhibitory neighborhood effects in lexical decision and semantic categorization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 22(3), 696–713. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.22.3.696
  • Frith, U. (1985). Beneath the surface of developmental dyslexia. In K. E. Patterson, J. C. Marashall & M. Coltheart (Eds.), Surface dyslexia (pp. 301–330). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
  • Grainger, J., Bouttevin, S., Truc, C., Bastien, M., & Ziegler, J. (2003). Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: A comparison of dyslexic and normal readers. Brain and Language, 87(3), 432–440. doi:10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00145-7
  • Grainger, J., & Jacobs, A. M. (1996). Orthographic processing in visual word recognition: A multiple read-out model. Psychological Review, 103(3), 518–565. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.3.518
  • Hofmann, M. J., Tamm, S., Braun, M. M., Dambacher, M., Hahne, A., & Jacobs, A. M. (2008). Conflict monitoring engages the mediofrontal cortex during nonword processing. Neuroreport, 19(1), 25–29. doi:10.1097/WNR.0b013e3282f3b134
  • Horie, S., Yamasaki, T., Okamoto, T., Nakashima, T., Ogata, K., & Tobimatsu, S. (2011). Differential roles of spatial frequency on reading processes for ideograms and phonograms: A high-density ERP study. Neurosci, 72(1), 68–78. Epub 2011 Oct 2014.
  • Hudson, P. T. W, & Bergman, M. W. (1985). Lexical knowledge in word recognition: Word length and word frequency in naming and lexical decision tasks. Journal of Memory and Language, 24(1), 46–58. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(85)90015-4
  • Ibrahim, M. H. (1983). Linguistic distance and literacy in Arabic. Journal of Pragmatics, 7(5), 507–515. doi:10.1016/0378-2166(83)90078-4
  • Ibrahim, R. (2008). Does visual and auditory word perception have a language-selective input? Evidence from word processing in Semitic languages. The Linguistics Journal, 3(2)
  • Ibrahim, R., Eviatar, Z., & Aharon-Peretz, J. (2002). The characteristics of Arabic orthography slow its processing. Neuropsychology, 16(3), 322–326. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.16.3.322
  • Khateb, A., Michel, C. M., Pegna, A. J., Landis, T., & Annoni, J.-M. (2000). New insights into the Stroop effect: A spatio-temporal analysis of electric brain activity. Neuroreport, 11(9), 1849–1855. doi:10.1097/00001756-200006260-00010
  • Khateb, A., Pegna, A. J., Michel, C. M., Landis, T., & Annoni, J.-M. (2002). Dynamics of brain activation during an explicit word and image recognition task: An electrophysiological study. Brain Topography, 14(3), 197–213. doi:10.1023/A:1014502925003
  • Levin, I., Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hende, N., & Ziv, M. (2008). Early literacy in Arabic: An intervention study among Israeli Palestinian kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29(03), 413–436. doi:10.1017/S0142716408080193
  • Maamouri, M. (1998). Arabic diglossia and its impact on the quality of education in the Arab region. A paper presented at The World Bank Mediterranean Development Forum, Marrakech, Morocco, September3, .
  • Massaro, D. W., & Cohen, M. M. (1994). Visual, orthographic, phonological, and lexical influences in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(6), 1107–1128. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.20.6.1107
  • Maurer, U., Brandeis, D., & McCandliss, B. D. (2005). Fast, visual specialization for reading in English revealed by the topography of the N170 ERP response. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 1(1), 13. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-1-13
  • Maurer, U., Brem, S., Bucher, K., Kranz, F., Benz, R., Steinhausen, H.-C., & Brandeis, D. (2007). Impaired tuning of a fast occipito-temporal response for print in dyslexic children learning to read. Brain, 130(12), 3200–3210. doi:10.1093/brain/awm193
  • McCarthy, R., & Warrington, E. K. (1990). Cognitive neuropsychology. San Diego, California: Academic PressDiego, California: Academic Press.
  • McClelland, J. L., & Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part I: An account of basic findings. Psychological Review, 88(5), 375–407. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375
  • Morton, J., & Frith, U. (1995). Causal modelling: A structural approach to developmental psychopathology. Manual of Developmental Psychopathology, 1, 357–390.
  • Naslund, J. C. (1990). The interrelationships among preschool predictors of reading acquisition for German children. Reading and Writing, 2(4), 327–360. doi:10.1007/BF00419387
  • Perfetti. (1985). Reading ability. In New York, NY: Oxford University Press
  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103(1), 56–115. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.56
  • Roman, G., & Pavard, B. (1987). A comparative study: How we read Arabic and French.Elsevier. In J. K. O'Regan & A. Levy-Schoen (Eds.), Eye movement: From physiology to cognition (pp. 431–440). Elsevier: Amsterdam, the Netherlands.
  • Rumelhart, D. E., & McClelland, J. L. (1982). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: Part 2. The contextual enhancement effect and some tests and extensions of the model. Psychological Review, 89(1), 60–94. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.89.1.60
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic Diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24(03), 431–451. doi:10.1017/S0142716403000225
  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of words and pseudo-words in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25(04), 495–512. doi:10.1017/S0142716404001249
  • Sereno, S. C., Rayner, K., Sereno, S. C., Brewer, C. C., & O'Donnell, P. J. (2003). Measuring word recognition in reading: Eye movements and event-related potentials. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 489–493. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.010
  • Shafrir, U., & Siegel, L. S. (1994). Subtypes of learning disabilities in adolescents and adults. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27(2), 123–124. doi:10.1177/002221949402700207
  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(94)00645-2
  • Siegel, L. S., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for Superior Orthographic Skills in Dyslexics. Psychological Science, 6(4), 250–254. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00601.x
  • Simon, G., Bernard, C., Largy, P., Lalonde, R., & Rebai, M. (2004). Chronometry of visual word recognition during passive and lexical decision tasks: An ERP investigation. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(21), 1–11. doi:10.1080/00207450490476057
  • Simon, G., Petit, L., Bernard, C., & Rebai, M. (2007). N170 ERPs could represent a logographic processing strategy in visual word recognition. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 3(1), 21. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-3-21
  • Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7(1), 37–46. doi:10.1002/dys.185
  • Snowling, M., & Hulme, C. (1994). The development of phonological skills. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 346(1315), 21–27. doi:10.1098/rstb.1994.0124
  • Stanovich. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual differences in the acquisition of literacy.
  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988). Explaining the differences between the dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21(10), 590–604. doi:10.1177/002221948802101003
  • Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (1989). Exposure to print and orthographic processing. Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), 402–433. doi:10.2307/747605
  • Stuart, M., Masterson, J., Dixon, M., & Quinlan, P. (1999). Inferring sublexical correspondences from sight vocabulary: Evidence from 6- and 7-year-olds. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 52(2), 353–366.
  • Taha, H., Ibrahim, R., & Khateb, A. (2012). How does Arabic orthographic connectivity modulate brain activity during visual word recognition: an ERP study. Brain Topogr, In press.
  • Temple, C. M. (1988). Red is read but eye is blue: A case study of developmental dyslexia and follow-up report. Brain and Language, 34(1), 13–37. doi:10.1016/0093-934X(88)90122-8
  • Vanderwood, M. L., Linklater, D., & Healy, K. (2008). Predictive accuracy of nonsense wordfluency for English language learners. School Psychology Review, 37, 5–17.
  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40. doi:10.1046/j.0021-9630.2003.00305.x
  • Vellutino, F. R., Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321–363. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23086538
  • Yap, M. J., Balota, D. A., Cortese, M. J., & Watson, J. M. (2006). Single- versus dual-process models of lexical decision performance: Insights from response time distributional analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 32(6), 1324–1344. doi:10.1037/0096-1523.32.6.1324
  • Ziegler, J. C., Bertrand, D., Toth, D., Csepe, V., Reis, A., Faisca, L., & Blomert, L. (2010). Orthographic depth and its impact on universal predictors of reading: A cross-language investigation. Psychological Science, 21(4), 551–559. doi:10.1177/0956797610363406

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.