730
Views
26
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Related but not the same: Ordinality, cardinality and 1-to-1 correspondence in finger-based numerical representations

, , &

REFERENCES

  • Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–645. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639
  • Barsalou, L. W. (2010). Grounded cognition: Past, present, and future. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 716–724. doi:10.1111/j.1756-8765.2010.01115.x
  • Beller, S., & Bender, A. (2011). Explicating numerical information: When and how fingers support (or hinder) number comprehension and handling. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognition, 2, 214. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00214
  • Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2012). Nature and culture of finger counting: Diversity and representational effects of an embodied cognitive tool. Cognition, 124, 156–182. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.05.005
  • Benton, A. L. (1959). Right-left discrimination and finger localization: Development and pathology. Oxford: Hoeber.
  • Bermejo, V. (1996). Cardinality development and counting. Developmental Psychology, 32, 263–268. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.32.2.263
  • Bermejo, V., & Lago, M. O. (1990). Developmental processes and stages in the acquisition of cardinality. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 13, 231–250. doi:10.1177/016502549001300206
  • Bermejo, V., Morales, S., & deOsuna, J. G. (2004). Supporting children's development of cardinality understanding. Learning and Instruction, 14, 381–398. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2004.06.010
  • Boulware, J. T. (1951). Numbness, body-image, and the Japanese illusion. Science, 114, 584–585. doi:10.1126/science.114.2970.584
  • Butterworth, B. (1999). The mathematical brain. London: Macmillan.
  • Chrisomalis, S. (2004). A cognitive typology for numerical notation. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 14 (1), 37–52. doi:10.1017/S0959774304000034
  • Davis, H., & Pérusse, R. (1988). Numerical competence in animals: Definitional issues, current evidence, and a new research agenda. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 11, 561–615. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00053437
  • Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122, 371. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
  • Delazer, M., & Butterworth, B. (1997). A dissociation of number meanings. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 14, 613–636. doi:10.1080/026432997381501
  • Deloche, G., & Seron, X. (1987). Numerical transcoding: A general production model. In Deloche, G. & Seron, X. (Eds). Mathematical disabilities: A cognitive neuropsychological perspective. Hillsdale, England: Lawrence Erlbaum.
  • Di Luca, S., Granà, A., Semenza, C., Seron, X., & Pesenti, M. (2006). Finger-digit compatibility in Arabic numeral processing. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1648–1663. doi:10.1080/17470210500256839
  • Di Luca, S., & Pesenti, M. (2008). Masked priming effect with canonical finger numeral configurations. Experimental Brain Research, 185, 27–39. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1132-8
  • Domahs, F., Klein, E., Moeller, K., Nuerk, H.-C., Yoon, B.-C., & Willmes, K. (2012). Multimodal semantic quantity representations: Further evidence from Korean sign language. Frontiers of Psychology, 2, 389. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00389
  • Domahs, F., Krinzinger, H., & Willmes, K. (2008). Mind the gap between both hands: Evidence for internal finger-based number representations in children's mental calculation. Cortex, 44, 359–367. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.001
  • Domahs, F., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Willmes, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2010). Embodied numerosity: implicit hand-based representations influence symbolic number processing across cultures. Cognition, 116, 251–266. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.05.007
  • Fayol, M, Barrouillet, P., & Marinthe, C. (1998). Predicting arithmetical achievement from neuropsychological performance: a longitudinal study. Cognition, 68, B63–B70. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00046-8
  • Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 307–314. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.05.002
  • Fischer, J. P. (2010). Numerical performance increased by finger training: A fallacy due to regression toward the mean? Cortex, 46, 272–273. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2008.06.010
  • Fischer, M. H. (2008). Finger counting habits modulate spatial-numerical associations. Cortex, 44, 386–392. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.004
  • Fischer, M. H., & Brugger, P. (2011). When digits help digits: spatial-number associations point to finger counting as prime examples of embodied cognition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 260. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00260
  • Fuson, K. C. (1988). Children's counting and concepts of number. New York: Springer.
  • Gelman, R., & Gallistel, C. R. (1978). The child's understanding of number. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of cognitive psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8, 573–585. doi:10.1177/1745691613498098
  • Gracia-Bafalluy, M., & Noël, M. P. (2008). Does finger training increase young children's numerical performance? Cortex, 44, 368–375. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2007.08.020
  • Jonas, C. N., & Ward, J. (2014). Number-space associations in synaesthesia are not influenced by finger-counting habits. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 232–240. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.866119
  • Klein, E., Moeller, K., Willmes, K., Nuerk, H.-C., & Domahs, F. (2011). The influence of implicit hand-based representations on mental arithmetic. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 197. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00197
  • Lindemann, O., Alipour, A., & Fischer, M. H. (2011). Finger counting habits in Middle Eastern and Western individuals: an online survey. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 42, 566–578. doi:10.1177/0022022111406254
  • Moeller, K., Fischer, U., Link, T. S., Wasner, M., Huber, S., Cress, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2012). Learning and development of embodied numerosity. Cognitive Processing, 13, 271–274. doi:10.1007/s10339-012-0457-9
  • Moeller, K., Martignon, L., Wessolowski, S., Engel, J., & Nuerk, H. C. (2011). Effects of finger counting on numerical development—The opposing views of neurocognition and mathematics education. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00328
  • Nieder, A. (2005). Counting on neurons: The neurobiology of numerical competence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 6, 177–190. doi:10.1038/nrn1626
  • Noël, M. P. (2005). Finger gnosia: A predictor of numerical abilities in children? Child Neuropsychology, 11, 413–430. doi:10.1080/09297040590951550
  • Overmann, K. A. (2014). Finger-counting in the Upper Palaeolithic. Rock Art Research, 31, 63–80. Retrieved from http://cat.inist.fr/?aModele=afficheN&cpsidt=28284198
  • Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2008). An alternative view of the relation between finger gnosis and math ability: Redeployment of finger representations for the representation of number. Proceedings of the 30th Annual Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Penner-Wilger, M., & Anderson, M. L. (2013). The relation between finger gnosis and mathematical ability: Why redeployment of neural circuits best explains the finding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 877. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00877
  • Penner-Wilger, M., Fast, L., LeFevre, J. A., Smith-Chant, B. L., Skwarchuk, S. L., Kamawar, D., & Bisanz, J. (2007). The foundations of numeracy: Subitizing, finger gnosia, and fine-motor ability. In D. S. McNamara & J. G. Trafton (Eds.), Proceedings of the 29th Annual Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1385–1390). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.
  • Pika, S., Nicoladis, E., & Marentette, P. (2008). How to order a beer: Cultural differences in the use of conventional gestures for numbers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40, 70–80. doi:10.1177/0022022108326197
  • Pixner, S., Moeller, K., Hermanova, V., Nuerk, H. C., & Kaufmann, L. (2011). Whorf reloaded: Language effects on nonverbal number processing in first grade—A trilingual study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 108, 371–382. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.002
  • Pulvermueller, F. (2013). How neurons make meaning: Brain mechanisms for embodied and abstract-symbolic semantics. Trends in Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 458–470. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.06.004
  • Riello, M., & Rusconi, E. (2011). Unimanual SNARC effect: hand matters. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 372. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00372
  • Rubinsten, O., & Sury, D. (2011). Processing ordinality and quantity: The case of developmental dyscalculia. PLoS ONE, 6, e24079. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024079
  • Rubinsten, O., Sury, D., Lavro, D., & Berger, A. (2013). Processing ordinality and quantity: ERP evidence of separate mechanisms. Brain and Cognition, 82, 201–212. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0024079
  • Saxe, G. B. (2012). Cultural development of mathematical ideas: the Papua New Guinea studies. New York: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9781139045360
  • Seron, X., & Fayol, M. (1994). Number transcoding in children: A functional analysis. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 12, 281–300. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1994.tb00635.x
  • Seron, X., & Noël, M. P. (1996). Transcoding numbers from the Arabic code to the verbal one or vice versa: How many routes? Mathematical Cognition, 1, 215–243.
  • Thevenot, C., Castel, C., Danjon, J., Renaud, O., Ballaz, C., Baggioni, L., & Fluss, J. (2014). Numerical abilities in children with congenital hemiplegia: An investigation of the role of finger use in number processing. Developmental Neuropsychology, 39, 88–100. doi:10.1080/87565641.2013.860979
  • Turconi, E., Jemel, B., Rossion, B., & Seron, X. (2004). Electrophysiological evidence for differential processing of numerical quantity and order in humans. Cognitive Brain Research, 21(1), 22–38. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.05.003
  • Wasner, M., Moeller, K., Fischer, M. H., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2014). Aspects of situated cognition in embodied numerosity: The case of finger counting. Cognitive Processing, 15, 317–328. doi:10.1007/s10339-014-0599-z
  • Widom, T. R., & Schlimm, D. (2012). Methodological reflections on typologies for numerical notations. Science in Context, 25, 155–195. doi:10.1017/S0269889712000038
  • Wiese, H. (2003). Numbers, language, and the human mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511486562
  • Wiese, H. (2007). The co-evolution of number concepts and counting words. Lingua, 117, 758–772. doi:10.1016/j.lingua.2006.03.001
  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–636. doi:10.3758/BF03196322
  • Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., & Willmes, K. (2006). Crossed hands and the SNARC effect: A failure to replicate Dehaene, Bossini and Girauy, 1993. Cortex, 42, 1069–1079. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70219-3
  • Zhang, J., & Norman, D. A. (1995). A representational analysis of numeration systems. Cognition, 57, 271–295. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00674-3
  • Zuber, J., Pixner, S., Moeller, K., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2009). On the language specificity of basic number processing: Transcoding in a language with inversion and its relation to working memory capacity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 60–77. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.003

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.