418
Views
11
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Vocal mistuning reveals the origin of musical scales

&
Pages 35-52 | Received 15 Jul 2015, Accepted 07 Dec 2015, Published online: 07 Jan 2016

References

  • Araya-Salas, M. (2012). Is birdsong music? Evaluating harmonic intervals in songs of a Neotropical songbird. Animal Behaviour, 84, 309–313. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.04.038
  • Arom, S., Fernando, N., & Marandola, F. (2007). An innovative method for the study of African musical scales: Cognitive and technical aspects. In C. Spyridis, A. Georgaki, G. Kouroupetroglou, & C. Anagnostopoulou (Eds.), Proceedings of the 4th sound and music computing conference (pp. 107–116). Athens: University of Athens.
  • Balzano, G. J. (1980). The group-theoretic description of 12-fold and microtonal pitch systems. Computer Music Journal, 4, 66–84. doi: 10.2307/3679467
  • Berkowska, M., & Dalla Bella, S. (2009). Reducing linguistic information enhances singing proficiency in occasional singers. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1169, 108–111. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04774.x
  • Berkowska, M., & Dalla Bella, S. (2013). Uncovering phenotypes of poor-pitch singing: The sung performance battery (SPB). Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 714. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00714
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2013). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 5.4.09) [Computer program]. Retrieved August 25, 2014, from http://www.praat.org/
  • Bregman, A. S. (1990). Auditory scene analysis: The perceptual organization of sound. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Burns, E. M. (1999). Intervals, scales, and tuning. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music ( 2nd ed., pp. 215–264). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Burns, E. M., & Ward, W. D. (1978). Categorical perception - phenomenon or epiphenomenon: Evidence from experiments in the perception of melodic musical intervals. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 63, 456–468. doi: 10.1121/1.381737
  • Cruttenden, A. (1997). Intonation (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Dalla Bella, S., Giguère, J. F., & Peretz, I. (2007). Singing proficiency in the general population. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 121, 1182–1189. doi: 10.1121/1.2427111
  • Daniélou, A. (1999). Introduction to the study of musical scales. New Delhi: Munshiram Manoharlal.
  • Devaney, J., & Ellis, D. P. W. (2008). An empirical approach to studying intonation tendencies in polyphonic vocal performances. Journal of Interdisciplinary Music Studies, 2, 141–56.
  • Devaney, J., Mandel, M. I., Ellis, D. P. W., & Fujinaga, I. (2011). Automatically extracting performance data from recordings of trained singers. Psychomusicology, 21, 108–136. doi: 10.1037/h0094008
  • Dowling, W. J. (1978). Scale and contour: Two components of a theory of memory for melodies. Psychological Review, 85, 341–354. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.85.4.341
  • Dowling, W. J., & Bartlett, J. C. (1981). The importance of interval information in long-term memory for melodies. Psychomusicology, 1, 30–49. doi: 10.1037/h0094275
  • Dowling, W. J., & Harwood, D. L. (1986). Music cognition. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Ellis, A. J. (1885). On the musical scales of various nations. Journal of the Society of Arts, 33, 485–527. Reprinted in K. Kaufman Shelemay (Ed.), Garland Library of Readings in Ethnomusicology 7 (pp. 1–43). New York: Garland, 1990.
  • Friberg, A., Bresin, R., & Sundberg, J. (2006). Overview of the KTH rule system for musical performance. Advances in Cognitive Psychology, 2–3, 145–161. doi: 10.2478/v10053-008-0052-x
  • Gill, K. Z., & Purves, D. (2009). A biological rationale for musical scales. PLoS One, 4, e8144. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0008144
  • Hagerman, B., & Sundberg, J. (1980). Fundamental frequency adjustment in barbershop singing. Journal of Research in Singing, 4, 3–17.
  • Handel, S. (1989). Listening: An introduction to the psychology of auditory events. Cambridge, MA: Bradford Books/MIT Press.
  • Hanson, H. (1960). Harmonic materials of modern music: Resources of the tempered scale. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
  • von Helmholtz, H. (1877/1954). On the sensations of tone as a physiological basis for the theory of music. New York: Dover.
  • Hillenbrand, J., Getty, L. A., Clark, M. J., & Wheeler, K. (1995). Acoustic characteristics of American English vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97, 3099–3111. doi: 10.1121/1.411872
  • Huron, D. (2006). Sweet anticipation: Music and the psychology of expectation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Huron, D. (2008). A comparison of average pitch height and interval size in major-and minor-key themes: Evidence consistent with affect-related pitch prosody. Empirical Musicology Review, 3, 59–63.
  • Hutchins, S., Roquet, C., & Peretz, I. (2012). The vocal generosity effect: How bad can your singing be? Music Perception, 30, 147–159. doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.30.2.147
  • Justus, T., & Hutsler, J. J. (2005). Fundamental issues in evolutionary psychology of music: Assessing innateness and domain specificity. Music Perception, 23, 1–27. doi: 10.1525/mp.2005.23.1.1
  • Klein, W., Plomp, R., & Pols, L. C. W. (1970). Vowel spectra, vowel spaces and vowel identification. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 48, 999–1009. doi: 10.1121/1.1912239
  • Krumhansl, C. (1990). Cognitive foundations of musical pitch. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Krumhansl, C. L., & Cuddy, L. (2010). A theory of tonal hierarchies in music. In M. R. Jones, R. R. Fay, & A. N. Popper (Eds.), Music perception (pp. 51–88). New York: Springer.
  • Ladd, D. R. (1984). Declination: A review and some hypotheses. Phonology Yearbook, 1, 53–74. doi: 10.1017/S0952675700000294
  • Ladd, D. R. (1996). Intonational phonology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  • Large, E. W. (2010). A dynamical systems approach to musical tonality. In R. Huys & V. K. Jirsa (Eds.), Nonlinear dynamics in human behavior (pp. 193–211). Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Larrouy-Maestri, P., & Morsomme, D. (2014a). Criteria and tools for objectively analysing the vocal accuracy of a popular song. Logopedics Phoniatrics Vocology, 39, 11–18. doi: 10.3109/14015439.2012.696139
  • Larrouy-Maestri, P., & Morsomme, D. (2014b). Effects of melody and technique on acoustical and musical features of western operatic singing voices. Journal of Voice, 28, 332–340. doi: 10.1016/j.jvoice.2013.10.019
  • Lomax, A. (1968). Folk song style and culture. Washington, DC: American Association for the Advancement of Science.
  • Loy, G. (2006). Musimathics: The mathematical foundations of music. Volume 1. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Mantell, J. T., & Pfordresher, P. Q. (2013). Vocal imitation of speech and song. Cognition, 127, 177–202. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.008
  • Moog, H. (1976). The musical experience of the pre-school child (C. Clarke, Trans.). London: Schott.
  • Morrison, S. J. and Fyk, J. (2002). Intonation. In R. Parncutt & G. McPherson (Eds.), The science & psychology of music performance: Creative strategies for teaching and learning (pp. 183–198). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Oxenham, A. J. (2013). The perception of musical tones. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music ( 3rd ed., pp. 1–33). Amsterdam: Academic Press.
  • Parncutt, R. (1989). Harmony: A psychoacoustical approach. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.
  • Parncutt, R. (2014). The emotional connotations of major versus minor tonality: One or more origins? Musicae Scientiae, 18, 324–353. doi: 10.1177/1029864914542842
  • Patel, A. D. (2008). Music, language, and the brain. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Peterson, G. E., & Barney, H. L. (1952). Control methods used in a study of the vowels. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 24, 175–184. doi: 10.1121/1.1906875
  • Pfordresher, P. Q., & Brown, S. (2007). Poor-pitch singing in the absence of “tone deafness”. Music Perception, 25, 95–115. doi: 10.1525/mp.2007.25.2.95
  • Pfordresher, P. Q., Brown, S., Meier, K. M., Belyk, M., & Liotti, M. (2010). Imprecise singing is widespread. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 128, 2182–2190. doi: 10.1121/1.3478782
  • Pfordresher, P. Q., & Larrouy-Maestri, P. (2015). On drawing a line through the spectrogram: How do we understand deficits of vocal pitch imitation? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 271. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00271
  • Plomp, R., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1965). Tonal consonance and critical bandwidth. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 38, 548–560. doi: 10.1121/1.1909741
  • Racette, A., & Peretz, I. (2007). Learning lyrics: To sing or not to sing? Memory & Cognition, 35, 242–253. doi: 10.3758/BF03193445
  • Smith, J. D., Kemler Nelson, D. G., Grohskopf, L. A., & Appleton, T. (1994). What child is this? What interval was that? Familiar tunes and music perception in novice listeners. Cognition, 52, 23–54. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(94)90003-5
  • Stevens, C., & Byron, T. (2009). Universals in music processing. In S. Hallam, I. Cross, & M. Thaut (Eds.), Oxford handbook of music psychology (pp. 14–23). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Sundberg, J. (2013). The perception of singing. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), The psychology of music ( 3rd ed., pp. 69–106). London: Academic Press.
  • Temperley, D., & Tan, D. (2013). Emotional connotations of diatonic modes. Music Perception, 30, 237–257. doi: 10.1525/mp.2012.30.3.237
  • Terhardt, E. (1984). The concept of musical consonance: A link between music and psychoacoustics. Music Perception, 1, 276–295. doi: 10.2307/40285261
  • Thompson, W. F. (2013). Intervals and scales. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of Music ( 3rd ed., pp. 107–140). Amsterdam: Academic Press.
  • Turner, R. E., & Patterson, R. D. (2003). An analysis of the size information in classical formant data: Peterson and Barney (1952) revisited. Journal of the Acoustical Society of Japan, 33, 585–589.
  • Vurma, A., & Ross, J. (2006). Production and perception of musical intervals. Music Perception, 23, 331–344. doi: 10.1525/mp.2006.23.4.331
  • Welch, G. (2006). Singing and vocal development. In G. McPherson (Ed.), The child as musician: A handbook of musical development (pp. 311–329). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Welch, G. F. (1979a). Poor pitch singing: A review of the literature. Psychology of Music, 7, 50–58. doi: 10.1177/030573567971006
  • Welch, G. F. (1979b). Vocal range and poor pitch singing. Psychology of Music, 7, 13–31. doi: 10.1177/030573567972002
  • Wise, K., & Sloboda, J. A. (2008). Establishing an empirical profile of self-defined ‘tone deafness’: Perception, singing performance, and self-assessment. Musicae Scientiae, 12, 3–23. doi: 10.1177/102986490801200102

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.