694
Views
17
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Executive control in older Welsh monolinguals and bilinguals

, , , , , , , , & show all
Pages 412-426 | Received 17 Nov 2014, Accepted 21 Jan 2016, Published online: 06 Apr 2016

References

  • Adesope, O. O., Lavin, T., Thompson, T., & Ungerleider, C. (2010). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the cognitive correlates of bilingualism. Review of Educational Research, 80, 207–245. doi:10.3102/0034654310368803.
  • Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Duggirala, V., Surampudi, B., Shailaja, M., Shukla, A. K., … Kaul, S. (2013). Bilingualism delays age at onset of dementia, independent of education and immigration status. Neurology, 81, 1938–1944. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000436620.33155.a4.
  • Alladi, S., Bak, T. H., Mekala, S., Rajan, A., Chaudhuri, J. R., Mioshi, E., … Kaul, S. (2016). Impact of bilingualism on cognitive outcome after stroke. Stroke, 47, 258–261. doi:10.1161/STROKEAHA.115.010418.
  • Baddeley, A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1992). The speed and capacity of language processing (SCOLP) test. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Baddeley, A. D., Emslie, H., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1993). The spot-the-word test: A robust estimate of verbal intelligence based on lexical decision. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 32, 55–65. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8260.1993.tb01027.x.
  • Bak, T. H. (2015). Beyond a simple “yes” and “no”. Cortex, 73, 332–333. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.08.003.
  • Bak, T. H., Nissan, J. J., Allerhand, M. M., & Deary, I. J. (2014). Does bilingualism influence cognitive aging? Annals of Neurology, 75, 959–963. doi:10.1002/ana.24158.
  • Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving toward a neuroplasticity view of bilingualism, executive control, and aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35, 857–894. doi:10.1017/s0142716414000174.
  • Bialystok, E. (2006). Effect of bilingualism and computer video game experience on the Simon task. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 60, 68–79. doi:10.1037/cjep2006008.
  • Bialystok, E. (2009). Claiming evidence from non-evidence: A reply to Morton and Harper. Developmental Science, 12, 499–501; discussion 502–503. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00868.x
  • Bialystok, E. (2011). Reshaping the mind: The benefits of bilingualism. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 229–235. doi:10.1037/a0025406.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Binns, M. A., Ossher, L., & Freedman, M. (2014). Effects of bilingualism on the age of onset and progression of MCI and AD: Evidence from executive function tests. Neuropsychology, 28, 290–304. doi:10.1037/neu0000023.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45, 459–464. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.10.009.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., Klein, R., & Viswanathan, M. (2004). Bilingualism, aging, and cognitive control: Evidence from the Simon task. Psychology and Aging, 19, 290–303. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.19.2.290.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859–873. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.34.4.859.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: Consequences for mind and brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 240–250. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.03.001.
  • Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Ryan, J. (2006). Executive control in a modified antisaccade task: Effects of aging and bilingualism. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 32, 1341–1354. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.32.6.1341.
  • Bialystok, E., Martin, M. M., & Viswanathan, M. (2005). Bilingualism across the lifespan: The rise and fall of inhibitory control. International Journal of Bilingualism, 9, 103–119. doi:10.1177/13670069050090010701.
  • Bialystok, E., & Viswanathan, M. (2009). Components of executive control with advantages for bilingual children in two cultures. Cognition, 112, 494–500. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.014.
  • de Bruin, A., Bak, T. H., & Della Sala, S. (2015). Examining the effects of active versus inactive bilingualism on executive control in a carefully matched non-immigrant sample. Journal of Memory and Language, 85, 15–26. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2015.07.001.
  • Buckner, R. L. (2004). Memory and executive function in aging and AD: Multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron, 44, 195–208. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006.
  • Clare, L., Whitaker, C. J., Craik, F. I. M., Bialystok, E., Martyr, A., Martin-Forbes, P. A., … Hindle, J. V. (2014). Bilingualism, executive control and age at diagnosis among people with early-stage Alzheimer's disease in Wales. Journal of Neuropsychology. doi:10.1111/jnp.12061
  • Costa, A., Hernandez, M., Costa-Faidella, J., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2009). On the bilingual advantage in conflict processing: Now you see it, now you don't. Cognition, 113, 135–149. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.08.001.
  • Costa, A., Hernandez, M., & Sebastian-Galles, N. (2008). Bilingualism aids conflict resolution: Evidence from the ANT task. Cognition, 106, 59–86. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2006.12.013.
  • Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2006a). Cognition through the lifespan: Mechanisms of change. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 131–138. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.007.
  • Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2006b). Planning and task management in older adults: Cooking breakfast. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1236–1249. doi:10.3758/BF03193268.
  • Dattalo, P. (2013). Analysis of multiple dependent variables. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
  • Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E., & Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis-Kaplan executive function system (D-KEFS). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Duñabeitia, J. A., Hernández, J. A., Antón, E., Macizo, P., Estévez, A., Fuentes, L., & Carreiras, M. (2014). The inhibitory advantage in bilingual children revisited: Myth or reality? Experimental Psychology, 61, 234–251. doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000243.
  • Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M., & Styles, B. (2009). British picture vocabulary scale (3rd ed.). London: GL Assessment.
  • Emmorey, K., Luk, G., Pyers, J. E., & Bialystok, E. (2008). The source of enhanced cognitive control in bilinguals: Evidence from bimodal bilinguals. Psychological Science, 19, 1201–1206. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02224.x.
  • Festman, J., Rodriguez-Fornells, A., & Munte, T. F. (2010). Individual differences in control of language interference in late bilinguals are mainly related to general executive abilities. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 6, 5. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-6-5.
  • Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189–198. doi:10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.
  • Gathercole, V. C. M. (Ed.). (2007). Language transmission in bilingual families in Wales. Cardiff: Welsh Language Board.
  • Gathercole, V. C. M., Kennedy, I., & Thomas, E. M. (2015). Socioeconomic level and bilinguals’ performance on language and cognitive measures. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 2015. doi:10.1017/S1366728915000504.
  • Gathercole, V. C. M., & Thomas, E. M. (2009). Bilingual first-language development: Dominant language takeover, threatened minority language take-up. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 213–237. doi:10.1017/S1366728909004015.
  • Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Jones, L., Viñas-Guasch, N., Young, N., & Hughes, E. K. (2010). Cognitive effects of bilingualism: Digging deeper for the contributions of language dominance, linguistic knowledge, socio-economic status and cognitive abilities. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13, 617–664. doi:10.1080/13670050.2010.488289.
  • Gathercole, V. C. M., Thomas, E. M., Kennedy, I., Prys, C., Young, N., Viñas-Guasch, N., … Jones, L. (2014). Does language dominance affect cognitive performance in Bilinguals? Lifespan evidence from preschoolers through older adults on Card Sorting, Simon, and Metalinguistic Tasks. Frontiers in Developmental Psychology, 5, 11. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00011.
  • Gold, B. T. (2015). Lifelong bilingualism and neural reserve against Alzheimer's disease: A review of findings and potential mechanisms. Behavioural Brain Research, 281, 9–15. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.12.006.
  • Gold, B. T., Kim, C., Johnson, N. F., Kryscio, R. J., & Smith, C. D. (2013). Lifelong bilingualism maintains neural efficiency for cognitive control in aging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 387–396. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3837-12.2013.
  • Gollan, T. H., Fennema-Notestine, C., Montoya, R. I., & Jernigan, T. L. (2007). The bilingual effect on Boston Naming Test performance. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 13, 197–208. doi:10.1017/S1355617707070038.
  • Gollan, T. H., Montoya, R. I., Cera, C., & Sandoval, T. C. (2008). More use almost always a means a smaller frequency effect: Aging, bilingualism, and the weaker links hypothesis. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 787–814. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.07.001.
  • Gollan, T. H., Sandoval, T., & Salmon, D. P. (2011). Cross-language intrusion errors in aging bilinguals reveal the link between executive control and language selection. Psychological Science, 22, 1155–1164. doi:10.1177/0956797611417002.
  • Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language control in bilinguals: The adaptive control hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 515–530. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.796377.
  • Hankee, L. D., Preis, S. R., Beiser, A. S., Devine, S. A., Liu, Y., Seshadri, S., … Au, R. (2013). Qualitative neuropsychological measures: Normative data on executive functioning tests from the Framingham offspring study. Experimental Aging Research, 39, 515–535. doi:10.1080/0361073X.2013.839029.
  • Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 625–658. doi:10.3758/s13423-011-0116-7.
  • Hindle, J. V., Martin-Forbes, P. A., Bastable, A. J., Pye, K. L., Martyr, A., Whitaker, C. J., … Clare, L. (2015). Cognitive reserve in Parkinson's disease: The effects of welsh-english bilingualism on executive function. Parkinson's Disease, 2015, 943572. doi:10.1155/2015/943572
  • Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). Boston naming test. Philadelphia, PA: Lea & Febiger.
  • Kirk, N. W., Fiala, L., Scott-Brown, K. C., & Kempe, V. (2014). No evidence for reduced Simon cost in elderly bilinguals and bidialectals. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26, 640–648. doi:10.1080/20445911.2014.929580.
  • Kousaie, S., & Phillips, N. A. (2012). Ageing and bilingualism: Absence of a “bilingual advantage” in Stroop interference in a nonimmigrant sample. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65, 356–369. doi:10.1080/17470218.2011.604788.
  • Kousaie, S., Sheppard, C., Lemieux, M., Monetta, L., & Taler, V. (2014). Executive function and bilingualism in young and older adults. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 250. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00250.
  • Lawton, M. P., & Brody, E. M. (1969). Assessment of older people: Self-maintaining and instrumental activities of daily living. The Gerontologist, 9, 179–186. doi:10.1093/geront/9.3_Part_1.179.
  • Lee, K., Ng, E. L., & Ng, S. F. (2009). The contributions of working memory and executive functioning to problem representation and solution generation in algebraic word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 373–387. doi:10.1037/A0013843.
  • Li, P., Legault, J., & Litcofsky, K. A. (2014). Neuroplasticity as a function of second language learning: Anatomical changes in the human brain. Cortex, 58, 301–324. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.05.001.
  • Ljungberg, J. K., Hansson, P., Andres, P., Josefsson, M., & Nilsson, L. G. (2013). A longitudinal study of memory advantages in bilinguals. PloS One, 8, e73029. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073029.
  • Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism is not a categorical variable: Interaction between language proficiency and usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 605–621. doi:10.1080/20445911.2013.795574.
  • Luk, G., Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Grady, C. L. (2011). Lifelong bilingualism maintains white matter integrity in older adults. The Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 16808–16813. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4563-11.2011.
  • Luo, L., Craik, F. I. M., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and Aging, 28, 28–34. doi:10.1037/a0030875.
  • Martyr, A., & Clare, L. (2012). Executive function and activities of daily living in Alzheimer's disease: A correlational meta-analysis. Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders, 33, 189–203. doi:10.1159/000338233.
  • McNab, F., Leroux, G., Strand, F., Thorell, L., Bergman, S., & Klingberg, T. (2008). Common and unique components of inhibition and working memory: An fMRI, within-subjects investigation. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2668–2682. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.04.023.
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458.
  • Morton, J. B., & Harper, S. N. (2007). What did Simon say? Revisiting the bilingual advantage. Developmental Science, 10, 719–726. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00623.x.
  • Nelson, H. E., & Willison, J. R. (1991). The revised national adult reading test: Test manual. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
  • Office for National Statistics. (2010). Standard occupational classification 2010. Volume 3. The national statistics socio-economic classification: (Rebased on the SOC2010) User Manual. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
  • Office for National Statistics. (2011). 2011 Census: aggregate data (England and Wales), Retrieved from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/census/2011/index.html
  • Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive Psychology, 66, 232–258. doi:10.1016/j.cogpsych.2012.12.002.
  • Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2015). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning either do not exist or are restricted to very specific and undetermined circumstances. Cortex, 69, 265–278. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2015.04.014.
  • Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: Problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 962. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962.
  • Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., & Fiestas, C. E. (2013). Development of bilingual semantic norms: Can two be one? In V. C. M. Gathercole (Ed.), Bilingual assessment solutions (pp. 103–124). Bristol: Multilingual Matters.
  • Robertson, I. H., Ward, T., Ridgeway, V., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1994). The test of everyday attention. Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company.
  • Royall, D. R. (1994). Precis of executive dyscontrol as a cause of problem behavior in dementia. Experimental Aging Research, 20, 73–94. doi:10.1080/03610739408253955.
  • Salthouse, T. A. (2005). Relations between cognitive abilities and measures of executive functioning. Neuropsychology, 19, 532–545. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.532.
  • Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: Evaluating the validity of the “use it or lose it” hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 68–87. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00005.x.
  • Simon, J. R. (1969). Reactions toward the source of stimulation. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 81, 174–176. doi:10.1037/h0027448.
  • Snaith, R. P., & Zigmond, A. S. (1994). HADS: Hospital anxiety and depression scale. Windsor: NFER-Nelson.
  • Stern, Y., Zarahn, E., Hilton, H. J., Flynn, J., DeLaPaz, R., & Rakitin, B. (2003). Exploring the neural basis of cognitive reserve. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 691–701. doi:10.1076/jcen.25.5.691.14573.
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651.
  • The EuroQol Group. (1990). EuroQol – a new facility for the measurement of health-related quality of life. Health Policy, 16, 199–208. doi:10.1016/0168-8510(90)90421-9.
  • Valenzuela, M. J., & Sachdev, P. (2006). Brain reserve and cognitive decline: A non-parametric systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 36, 1065–1073. doi:10.1017/S0033291706007744.
  • Valenzuela, M. J., & Sachdev, P. (2007). Assessment of complex mental activity across the lifespan: Development of the lifetime of experiences questionnaire (LEQ). Psychological Medicine, 37, 1015–1025. doi:10.1017/s003329170600938x.
  • Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18, 3–24. doi:10.1017/S1366728914000522.
  • Vega-Mendoza, M., West, H., Sorace, A., & Bak, T. H. (2015). The impact of late, non-balanced bilingualism on cognitive performance. Cognition, 137, 40–46. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2014.12.008.
  • Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler memory scale (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.
  • Weissberger, G. H., Wierenga, C. E., Bondi, M. W., & Gollan, T. H. (2012). Partially overlapping mechanisms of language and task control in young and older bilinguals. Psychology and Aging, 27, 959–974. doi:10.1037/a0028281.
  • Welsh Government. (2012). 2011 census: First results on the Welsh language. Cardiff: Welsh Government.
  • Williams, J. W., Plassman, B. L., Burke, J., Holsinger, T., & Benjamin, S. (2010). Preventing Alzheimer's disease and cognitive decline. Durham, NC: Prepared by Duke Evidence-Based Practice Center for the U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality.
  • Yntema, D. B., & Mueser, G. E. (1962). Keeping track of variables that have few or many states. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 391–395. doi:10.1037/h0045706.
  • Zied, K. M., Phillipe, A., Pinon, K., Havet-Thomassin, V., Aubin, G., Roy, A., & Le Gall, D. (2004). Bilingualism and adult differences in inhibitory mechanisms: Evidence from a bilingual stroop task. Brain and Cognition, 54, 254–256. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.036.

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.