647
Views
10
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

The effects of implementation intention strategies on prospective memory cue encoding

, &
Pages 929-938 | Received 23 Aug 2016, Accepted 04 May 2017, Published online: 23 May 2017

References

  • Altgassen, M., Rendell, P. G., Bernhard, A., Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., Phillips, L. H., & Kliegel, M. (2015). Future thinking improves prospective memory performance and plan enactment in older adults. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 68(1), 192–204. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.956127
  • Brewer, G. A., Knight, J., Meeks, J. T., & Marsh, R. L. (2011). On the role of imagery in event-based prospective memory. Consciousness and Cognition, 20(3), 901–907. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2011.02.015
  • Brewer, G. A., & Marsh, R. L. (2010). On the role of episodic future simulation in encoding of prospective memories. Cognitive Neuroscience, 1(2), 81–88. doi: 10.1080/17588920903373960
  • Bugg, J. M., Scullin, M. K., & McDaniel, M. A. (2013). Strengthening encoding via implementation intention formation increases prospective memory commission errors. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(3), 522–527. doi: 10.3758/s13423-013-0378-3
  • Burkard, C., Rochat, L., & Van der Linden, M. (2013). Enhancing inhibition: How impulsivity and emotional activation interact with different implementation intentions. Acta Psychologica, 144(2), 291–297. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.07.008
  • Chen, X. J., Wang, Y., Liu, L. L., Cui, J. F., Gan, M. Y., Shum, D. H., & Chan, R. C. (2015). The effect of implementation intention on prospective memory: A systematic and meta-analytic review. Psychiatry Research, 226(1), 14–22. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2015.01.011
  • Chen, X. J., Wang, Y., Liu, L. L., Shi, H. S., Wang, J., Cui, J. F., … Chan, R. C. (2014). The effect and mechanisms of implementation intentions on prospective memory in individuals with and without schizotypal personality features. Memory, 22(4), 349–359. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.792841
  • Cohen, A.-L., & Gollwitzer, P. M. (2008). The cost of remembering to remember: Cognitive load and implementation intentions influence ongoing task performance. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 367–390). New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Einstein, G. O., & McDaniel, M. A. (2005). Prospective memory: Multiple retrieval processes. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 14(6), 286–290. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00382.x
  • Ellis, J. A., & Freeman, J. E. (2008). Ten years on: Realizing delayed intentions. In M. Kliegel, M. A. McDaniel, & G. O. Einstein (Eds.), Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives (pp. 1–28). Hove: Psychology Press.
  • Ellis, J., & Milne, A. (1996). Retrieval cue specificity and the realization of delayed intentions. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A, 49(4), 862–887. doi: 10.1080/713755662
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146
  • Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493
  • Kliegel, M., Martin, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2001). Varying the importance of a prospective memory task: Differential effects across time- and event-based prospective memory. Memory, 9(1), 1–11. doi: 10.1080/09658210042000003
  • Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2000). Plan formation, retention, and execution in prospective memory: A new approach and age-related effects. Memory & Cognition, 28(6), 1041–1049. doi: 10.3758/BF03209352
  • Kliegel, M., McDaniel, M. A., & Einstein, G. O. (2008). Prospective memory: Cognitive, neuroscience, developmental, and applied perspectives. New York: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
  • Knäuper, B., Roseman, M., Johnson, P. J., & Krantz, L. H. (2009). Using mental imagery to enhance the effectiveness of implementation intentions. Current Psychology, 28, 181–186. doi: 10.1007/s12144-009-9055-0
  • McCrea, S. M., Penningroth, S. L., & Radakovich, M. P. (2015). Implementation intentions forge a strong cue–response link and boost prospective memory performance. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 27(1), 12–26. doi: 10.1080/20445911.2014.975816
  • McDaniel, M. A., Howard, D. C., & Butler, K. M. (2008). Implementation intentions facilitate prospective memory under high attention demands. Memory & Cognition, 36, 716–724. doi: 10.3758/MC.36.4.716
  • McDaniel, M. A., Robinson-Riegler, B., & Einstein, G. O. (1998). Prospective remembering: Perceptually driven or conceptually driven processes? Memory & Cognition, 26(1), 121–134. doi: 10.3758/BF03211375
  • McDaniel, M. A., & Scullin, M. K. (2010). Implementation intention encoding does not automatize prospective memory responding. Memory & Cognition, 38(2), 221–232. doi: 10.3758/MC.38.2.221
  • McFarland, C., & Glisky, E. (2012). Implementation intentions and imagery: Individual and combined effects on prospective memory among young adults. Memory & Cognition, 40(1), 62–69. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0126-8
  • Meeks, J. T., & Marsh, R. L. (2010). Implementation intentions about nonfocal event-based prospective memory tasks. Psychological Research, 74(1), 82–89. doi: 10.1007/s00426-008-0223-x
  • Mäntyla, T. (1993). Priming effects in prospective memory. Memory, 1, 203–218. doi: 10.1080/09658219308258233
  • Nowinski, J. L., & Dismukes, K. (2005). Effects of ongoing task context and target typicality on prospective memory performance: The importance of associative cueing. Memory, 13(6), 649–657. doi: 10.1080/09658210444000313
  • Reese, C. M., & Cherry, K. E. (2002). The effects of age, ability, and memory monitoring on prospective memory task performance. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 9(2), 98–113. doi: 10.1076/anec.9.2.98.9546
  • Rummel, J., Einstein, G. O., & Rampey, H. (2012). Implementation-intention encoding in a prospective memory task enhances spontaneous retrieval of intentions. Memory, 20(8), 803–817. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2012.707214
  • Rummel, J., & Meiser, T. (2013). The role of metacognition in prospective memory: Anticipated task demands influence attention allocation strategies. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(3), 931–943. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.06.006
  • Scullin, M. K., Dasse, M., McDaniel, M. A., Nguyen, K., Lee, J., Kurinec, C., … Krueger, M. (2017). About the encoding of prospective memory intentions. Manuscript under review.
  • Shelton, J. T., Lee, J. H., Scullin, M. K., Rose, N. S., Rendell, P. G., & McDaniel, M. A. (2016). Improving prospective memory in healthy older adults and individuals with very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 64(6), 1307–1312. doi: 10.1111/jgs.14134
  • Smith, R. E., McConnell Rogers, M. D., McVay, J. C., Lopez, J. A., & Loft, S. (2014). Investigating how implementation intentions improve non-focal prospective memory tasks. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 213–230. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.05.003
  • Szpunar, K. K., Khan, N. Y., & Schacter, D. L. (2013). Interpolated memory tests reduce mind wandering and improve learning of online lectures. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(16), 6313–6317. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1221764110
  • Terrett, G., Rose, N. S., Henry, J. D., Bailey, P. E., Altgassen, M., Phillips, L. H., … Rendell, P. G. (2016). The relationship between prospective memory and episodic future thinking in younger and older adulthood. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 310–323. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2015.1054294
  • Terry, W. S. (1988). Everyday forgetting: Data from a diary study. Psychological Reports, 62, 299–303. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1988.62.1.299
  • Van Overschelde, J. P., Rawson, K. A., & Dunlosky, J. (2004). Category norms: An updated and expanded version of the Battig and Montague (1969) norms. Journal of Memory and Language, 50(3), 289–335. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2003.10.003
  • Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2008). Mechanisms of implementation intention effects: The role of goal intentions, self-efficacy, and accessibility of plan components. British Journal of Social Psychology, 47(3), 373–395. doi: 10.1348/014466607X267010
  • Zimmermann, T. D., & Meier, B. (2010). The effect of implementation intentions on prospective memory performance across the lifespan. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24(5), 645–658. doi: 10.1002/acp.1576

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.