1,625
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Original Articles

Spatial offloading in multiple task monitoring

, , , &
Pages 230-241 | Received 09 Jun 2017, Accepted 23 Jan 2018, Published online: 06 Feb 2018

References

  • Bonato, M., Zorzi, M., & Umiltà, C. (2012). When time is space: Evidence for a mental time line. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 36, 2257–2273. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.007
  • Bratfisch, O., & Hagman, E. (2003). Simultankapazität/Multi-Tasking (SIMKAP) Version 24.00: Handanweisung [Simultaneous capacity/multi-tasking (SIMKAP) release 24.00: Manual]. Mödling: Schuhfried.
  • Burgess, P. W., Veitch, E., De Lacy Costello, A., & Shallice, T. (2000). The cognitive and neuroanatomical correlates of multitasking. Neuropsychologia, 38, 848–863. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00134-7
  • Carretti, B., Cornoldi, C., DeBeni, R., & Roman, M. (2005). Updating in working memory: A comparison of poor and good comprehenders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 91, 45–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2005.01.005
  • Casasanto, D., & Boroditsky, L. (2008). Time in the mind: Using space to think about time. Cognition, 106, 579–593. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2007.03.004
  • Chisholm, C. D., Dornfeld, A. M., Nelson, D. R., & Cordell, W. H. (2001). Work interrupted: A comparison of workplace interruptions in emergency departments and primary care offices. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 38, 146–151. doi: 10.1067/mem.2001.115440
  • Clark, A. (2010). Supersizing the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.
  • Craik, F. I. M., & Bialystok, E. (2006). Planning and task management in older adults: Cooking breakfast. Memory & Cognition, 34, 1236–1249. doi: 10.3758/BF03193268
  • Dehaene, S., & Brannon, E. M. (2011). Space, time and number in the brain: Searching for the foundations of mathematical thought. London: Elsevier.
  • Del Missier, F., Mäntylä, T., & Bruin, W. B. (2012). Decision-making competence, executive functioning, and general cognitive abilities. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 25, 331–351. doi: 10.1002/bdm.731
  • Fisher, H. (1999). The first sex: The natural talents of women and how they are changing the world. New York, NY: Random House.
  • Fuster, J. M. (2001). The prefrontal cortex - an update: Time is of the essence. Neuron, 30, 319–333. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(01)00285-9
  • Gijssels, T., Bottini, R., Rueschemeyer, S.-A., & Casasanto, D. (2013). Space and time in the parietal cortex: fMRI evidence for a neural asymmetry. Proceedings of the 35th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (CogSci 2013), 4, 495–500.
  • Goldinger, S. D., Papesh, M. H., Barnhart, A. S., Hansen, W. A., & Hout, M. C. (2016). The poverty of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23, 959–978. doi: 10.3758/s13423-015-0860-1
  • Halpern, D. F. (2012). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (4th ed.). New York, NY: Psychology Press.
  • Hampson, E., Levy-Cooperman, N., & Korman, J. M. (2014). Estradiol and mental rotation: Relation to dimensionality, difficulty, or angular disparity? Hormones and Behavior, 65, 238–248. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2013.12.016
  • Hampson, E., & Morley, E. E. (2013). Estradiol concentrations and working memory performance in women of reproductive age. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 38, 2897–2904. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2013.07.020
  • Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the Wild. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • Janczyk, M., Renas, S., & Durst, M. (2017). Identifying the locus of compatibility-based backward crosstalk: Evidence from an extended PRP paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology : Human Perception and Performance, 49(0), 1–49.
  • Kirsh, D., & Maglio, P. (1994). On distinguishing epistemic from pragmatic action. Cognitive Science, 18, 513–549. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1804_1
  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1987). Seeing and imagining in the cerebral hemispheres: Computational approach. Psychological Review, 94, 148–175. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.94.2.148
  • Laeng, B., & Chabris, C. F. (2003). Asymmetries in encoding spatial relations. In K. Hugdahl & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The asymmetrical brain (pp. 303–309). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Logan, G. D. (1994). On the ability to inhibit thought and action: A users’ guide to the stop signal paradigm. In D. Dagenbach, & T. H. Carr (Eds.), Inhibitory processes in attention, memory, and language (pp. 189–239). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
  • Logie, R. H., Trawley, S., & Law, A. S. (2011). Multitasking: Multiple, domain-specific cognitive functions in a virtual environment. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1561–1574. doi: 10.3758/s13421-011-0120-1
  • Loukopoulos, L. D., Dismukes, R. K., & Barshi, I. (2009). The multitasking myth: Handling complexity in real-world operations. Farnham: Ashgate.
  • Mackinlay, R., Charman, T., & Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2006). High functioning children with autism spectrum disorder: A novel test of multitasking. Brain and Cognition, 61(1), 14–24. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2005.12.006
  • Maki, P. M., Rich, J. B., & Shayna Rosenbaum, R. (2002). Implicit memory varies across the menstrual cycle: Estrogen effects in young women. Neuropsychologia, 40, 518–529. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00126-9
  • Mäntylä, T. (2013). Gender differences in multitasking reflect spatial ability. Psychological Science, 24(5), 514–520. doi: 10.1177/0956797612459660
  • Mäntylä, T., Carelli, M. G., & Forman, H. (2007). Time monitoring and executive functioning in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 96, 1–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2006.08.003
  • Mäntylä, T., Coni, V., Kubik, V., Todorov, I., & Del Missier, F. (2017). Time takes space: Selective effects of multitasking on concurrent spatial processing. Cognitive Processing, 18, 229–235. doi: 10.1007/s10339-017-0799-4
  • Mäntylä, T., Del Missier, F., & Nilsson, L. G. (2009). Age differences in multiple outcome measures of time-based prospective memory. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 16, 708–720. doi: 10.1080/13825580902912721
  • Mäntylä, T., & Todorov, I. (2013). Questioning anecdotal beliefs and scientific findings. Psychological Science, 24, 811–812. doi: 10.1177/0956797612475319
  • Meyer, D. E., & Kieras, D. E. (1997). A computational theory of executive cognitive processes and multiple-task performance: Part 1. Basic mechanisms. Psychological Review, 104, 3–65. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.104.1.3
  • Miller, D. I., & Halpern, D. F. (2014). The new science of cognitive sex differences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18, 37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.10.011
  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. doi: 10.1177/0963721411429458
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. doi: 10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Morgan, B., D’Mello, S., Abbott, R., Radvansky, G., Haass, M., & Tamplin, A. (2013). Individual differences in multitasking ability and adaptability. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 55, 776–788. doi: 10.1177/0018720812470842
  • Morris, N., & Jones, D. M. (1990). Memory updating in working memory: The role of the central executive. British Journal of Psychology, 81, 111–121. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1990.tb02349.x
  • Navon, D., & Gopher, D. (1979). On the economy of the human-processing system. Psychological Review, 86, 214–255. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.86.3.214
  • Norman, D. A., & Shallice, T. (1986). Attention to action: Willed and automatic control of behaviour (Revised reprint of Norman and Shallice (1980)). In R. J. Davidson, G. E. Schwartz, & D. Shapiro (Eds.), Consciousness and self-regulation: Advances in research and theory (pp. 1–18). Boston, MA: Springer.
  • Nunes, A., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Experience-based mitigation of age-related performance declines: Evidence from air traffic control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 12–24.
  • Núñez, R., & Cooperrider, K. (2013). The tangle of space and time in human cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(5), 220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.008
  • Pashler, H. (1994). Dual-task interference in simple tasks: Data and theory. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 220–244. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.220
  • Pease, A., & Pease, B. (2003). Why men can only do one thing at a time and women never stop talking. Bhopal: Orion.
  • Peters, E., Hess, T. M., Västfjäll, D., & Auman, C. (2007). Adult age differences in dual information processes: Implications for the role of affective and deliberative processes in older adults’ decision making. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 2, 1–23. doi: 10.1111/j.1745-6916.2007.00025.x
  • Peters, M., Laeng, B., Latham, K., Jackson, M., Zaiyouna, R., & Richardson, C. (1995). A redrawn Vandenberg and Kuse mental rotations test-different versions and factors that affect performance. Brain and Cognition, 28, 39–58. doi: 10.1006/brcg.1995.1032
  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Meier, M. E., Montroy, J. J., Hicks, K. L., Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Cognitive predictors of a common multitasking ability: Contributions from working memory, attention control, and fluid intelligence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142, 359–379. doi: 10.1037/a0029082
  • Risko, E. F., & Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Cognitive offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 676–688. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.002
  • Risko, E. F., Medimorec, S., Chisholm, J., & Kingstone, A. (2014). Rotating with rotated text: A natural behavior approach to investigating cognitive offloading. Cognitive Science, 38(3), 537–564. doi: 10.1111/cogs.12087
  • Salthouse, T. A., Atkinson, T. M., & Berish, D. E. (2003). Executive functioning as a potential mediator of age-related cognitive decline in normal adults. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 566–594. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.132.4.566
  • Salvucci, D. D. (2005). A multitasking general executive for compound continuous tasks. Cognitive Science, 29, 457–492. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog0000_19
  • Salvucci, D. D., & Taatgen, N. A. (2008). Threaded cognition: An integrated theory of concurrent multitasking. Psychological Review, 115, 101–130. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.115.1.101
  • Shallice, T., & Burgess, P. (1991). Deficits in strategy application following frontal lobe damage in man. Brain, 114, 727–741. doi: 10.1093/brain/114.2.727
  • Shipstead, Z., Harrison, T. L., & Engle, R. W. (2016). Working memory capacity and fluid intelligence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 771–799. doi: 10.1177/1745691616650647
  • Stocker, K. (2014). The theory of cognitive spacetime. Metaphor and Symbol, 29(2), 71–93. doi: 10.1080/10926488.2014.889991
  • Stoet, G., O’Connor, D. B., Conner, M., & Laws, K. R. (2013). Are women better than men at multi-tasking? BMC Psychology, 1, 18–28. doi: 10.1186/2050-7283-1-18
  • Strayer, D. L., Medeiros-Ward, N., & Watson, J. M. (2013). Gender invariance in multitasking: A comment on Mäntylä (2013). Psychological Science, 24, 809–810. doi: 10.1177/0956797612465199
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Stroop color word test. Journal of Experimental Physiology, 18, 643–662.
  • Szameitat, A. J., Lepsien, J., Von Cramon, D. Y., Sterr, A., & Schubert, T. (2006). Task-order coordination in dual-task performance and the lateral prefrontal cortex: An event-related fMRI study. Psychological Research, 70(6), 541–552. doi: 10.1007/s00426-005-0015-5
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Cleaning up your act: Screening data prior to analysis. Using Multivariate Statistics, 5, 61–116.
  • Todorov, I., Del Missier, F., Konke, L. A., & Mäntylä, T. (2015). Deadlines in space: Selective effects of coordinate spatial processing in multitasking. Memory & Cognition, 43, 1216–1228. doi: 10.3758/s13421-015-0529-z
  • Todorov, I., Del Missier, F., & Mäntylä, T. (2014). Age-related differences in multiple task monitoring. PloS ONE, 9(9), e107619. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0107619
  • Vandenberg, S. G., & Kuse, A. R. (1978). Mental rotations, a group test of three-dimensional spatial visualization. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 47, 599–604. doi: 10.2466/pms.1978.47.2.599
  • Walsh, V. (2003). A theory of magnitude: Common cortical metrics of time, space and quantity. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(11), 483–488. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.002
  • Wickens, C. D. (2002). Multiple resources and performance prediction. Theoretical Issues in Ergonomics Science, 3, 159–177. doi: 10.1080/14639220210123806
  • Wickens, C. D. (2008). Multiple resources and mental workload. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 50, 449–455. doi: 10.1518/001872008X288394
  • Wilhelm, O., Hildebrandt, A., & Oberauer, K. (2013). What is working memory capacity, and how can we measure it? Frontiers in Psychology, 4(433), 1–22.
  • Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 9, 625–636. doi: 10.3758/BF03196322
  • Wilson, A. D., & Golonka, S. (2013). Embodied cognition is not what you think it is. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 263–275. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00058
  • Zhang, J., & Norman, D. A. (1994). Representations in distributed cognitive tasks. Cognitive Science, 18(1), 87–122. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog1801_3