1,176
Views
3
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Transferability and sustainability of task-switching training in socioeconomically disadvantaged children: a randomized experimental study

ORCID Icon, , & ORCID Icon
Pages 747-763 | Received 22 Mar 2020, Accepted 14 Oct 2020, Published online: 02 Nov 2020

References

  • Ackerman, D. J., & Friedman-Krauss, A. H. (2017). Preschoolers’ executive function: Importance, contributors, research needs and assessment options. ETS Research Report Series, 1(1), 1–24. https://doi.org/10.1002/ets2.12148
  • Alloway, T. P., Bibile, V., & Lau, G. (2013). Computerized working memory training: Can it lead to gains in cognitive skills in students? Computers in Human Behavior, 29(3), 632–638. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2012.10.023
  • Anguera, J. A., Boccanfuso, J., Rintoul, J. L., Al-Hashimi, O., Faraji, F., Janowich, J., Kong, E., …, & Gazzaley, A (2013). Video game training enhances cognitive control in older adults. Nature, 501(7465), 97–101. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12486
  • Arán Filippetti, V., & Richaud, M. C. (2017). A structural equation modeling of executive functions, IQ and mathematical skills in primary students: Differential effects on number production, mental calculus and arithmetical problems. Child Neuropsychology, 23(7), 864–888. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2016.1199665
  • Bennett, S. J., Holmes, J., & Buckley, S. (2013). Computerized memory training leads to sustained improvement in visuospatial short-term memory skills in children with Down syndrome. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 118(3), 179–192. https://doi.org/10.1352/1944-7558-118.3.179
  • Berryhill, M. E., & Hughes, H. C. (2009). On the minimization of task switch costs following long-term training. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(3), 503–514. https://doi.org/10.3758/APP.71.3.503
  • Bherer, L., Kramer, A. F., Peterson, M. S., Colcombe, S., Erickson, K., & Becic, E. (2008). Transfer effects in task-set cost and dual-task cost after dual-task training in older and younger adults: Further evidence for cognitive plasticity in attentional control in late adulthood. Experimental Aging Research, 34(3), 188–219. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610730802070068
  • Birgisdóttir, F., Gestsdóttir, S., & Thorsdóttir, F. (2015). The role of behavioral self-regulation in learning to read: A 2-year longitudinal study of Icelandic preschool children. Early Education and Development, 26(5-6), 807–828. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1003505
  • Blair, C. (2016). Executive function and early childhood education. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 10, 102–107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.05.009
  • Blair, C., Raver, C. C., Granger, D., Mills-Koonce, R., Hibel, L., & Family Life Project Key Investigators. (2011). Allostasis and allostatic load in the context of poverty in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 23(3), 845–857. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579411000344
  • Boot, W. R., Simons, D. J., Stothart, C., & Stutts, C. (2013). The pervasive problem with placebos in psychology: Why active control groups are not sufficient to rule out placebo effects. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8(4), 445–454. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613491271
  • Bridgett, D. J., Oddi, K. B., Laake, L. M., Murdock, K. W., & Bachmann, M. N. (2013). Integrating and differentiating aspects of self-regulation: Effortful control, executive functioning, and links to negative affectivity. Emotion, 13(1), 47–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029536
  • Buitenweg, J. I., Murre, J. M., & Ridderinkhof, K. R. (2012). Brain training in progress: A review of trainability in healthy seniors. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 183. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00183
  • Cantin, R. H., Gnaedinger, E. K., Gallaway, K. C., Hesson-McInnis, M. S., & Hund, A. M. (2016). Executive functioning predicts reading, mathematics, and theory of mind during the elementary years. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 146, 66–78. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.01.014
  • Cepeda, N. J., Kramer, A. F., & Gonzalez de Sather, J. C.M. (2001). Changes in executive control across the life span: Examination of task-switching performance. Developmental Psychology, 37(5), 715–730. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.37.5.715
  • Chen, J. J. L. (2005). Relation of academic support from parents, teachers, and peers to Hong Kong adolescents’ academic achievement: The mediating role of academic engagement. Genetic, Social, and General Psychology Monographs, 131(2), 77–127. https://doi.org/10.3200/MONO.131.2.77-127
  • Chen, J. J. L. (2008). Grade-level differences: Relations of parental, teacher and peer support to academic engagement and achievement among Hong Kong students. School Psychology International, 29(2), 183–198. https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034308090059
  • Chen, S. H., Main, A., Zhou, Q., Bunge, S. A., Lau, N., & Chu, K. (2015). Effortful control and early academic achievement of Chinese American children in immigrant families. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 30, 45–56. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2014.08.004
  • Chung, K. K. H., Liu, H., McBride, C., Wong, A. M., & Lo, J. C. M. (2016). How socioeconomic status, executive functioning and verbal interactions contribute to early academic achievement in Chinese children. Educational Psychology, 37, 1–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/01443410.2016.1179264
  • Clark, C. A. C., Nelson, J. M., Garza, J., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2014). Gaining control: Changing relations between executive control and processing speed and their relevance for mathematics achievement over course of the preschool period. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 107. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00107
  • Clark, C. A., Pritchard, V. E., & Woodward, L. J. (2010). Preschool executive functioning abilities predict early mathematics achievement. Developmental Psychology, 46(5), 1176–1191. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019672
  • Clark, C. A., Sheffield, T. D., Wiebe, S. A., & Espy, K. A. (2013). Longitudinal associations between executive control and developing mathematical competence in preschool boys and girls. Child Development, 84(2), 662–677. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01854.x
  • de Rosa Piccolo, L., Arteche, A. X., Fonseca, R. P., Grassi-Oliveira, R., & Salles, J. F. (2016). Influence of family socioeconomic status on IQ, language, memory, and executive functions of Brazilian children. Psicologia Reflexão e Crítica, 29(1), 23. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41155-016-0016-x
  • Diamond, A. (2012). Activities and programs that improve children’s executive functions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(5), 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721412453722
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
  • Diamond, A., Barnet, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387–1388. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1151148
  • Eisenberg, N., Smith, C. L., Sadovsky, A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2004). Effortful control: Relations with emotion regulation, adjustment, and socialization in childhood. In R. F. Baumeister, & K. D. Vohs (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (pp. 259–282). The Guilford Press.
  • Endres, M., Houpt, J. W., Donkin, C., & Finn, R. R. (2015). Working memory capacity and redundant information processing efficiency. Froniters in Psychology, 6, 594. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00594
  • Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28(1), 1–11. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03203630
  • Evans, G. W., & Kim, P. (2012). Childhood poverty, chronic stress, self-regulation, and coping. Child Development Perspectives, 7(1), 42–48. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024768
  • Fuhs, M. W., Farran, D. C., & Nesbitt, K. T. (2015). Prekindergarten children’s executive functioning skills and achievement gains: The utility of direct assessments and teacher ratings. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107(1), 207–221. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037366
  • Gerst, E. H., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., & Yoshida, H. (2017). Cognitive and behavioral rating measures of executive function as predictors of academic outcomes in children. Child Neuropsychology, 23(4), 381–407. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2015.1120860
  • Gibson, B. S., Gondoli, D. M., Johnson, A. C., Steeger, C. M., Dobrzenski, B. A., & Morrissey, R. A. (2011). Component analysis of verbal versus spatial working memory training in adolescents with ADHD: A randomized, controlled trial. Child Neuropsychology, 17(6), 546–563. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.551186
  • Golden, C. J. (1987). Manual for the stroop color and word test. Stoelting Co.
  • Green, C. S., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2014). On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychological Research, 78(6), 756–772. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0535-3
  • Hackman, D. A., Farah, M. J., & Meaney, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status and the brain: Mechanistic insights from human and animal research. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(9), 651–659. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2897
  • Haft, S. L., & Hoeft, F. (2017). Poverty’s impact on children executive functions: Global considerations. New Directions for Child and Adolescent, 158(158), 69–79. https://doi.org/10.1002/cad.20220
  • Hebb, D. O. (1949). The organization of behavior: A neurophysiological theory. Wiley.
  • Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Developmental Science, 12(4), F9–F15. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x
  • Hong Kong Psychological Society. (2020). http://www.hkps.org.hk/en/.
  • Hsu, N. S., Novick, J. M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2014). The development and malleability of executive control abilities. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 221. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00221
  • Jacob, R., & Parkinson, J. (2015). The potential for school-based interventions that target executive function to improve academic achievement: A review. Review of Educational Research, 85(4), 512–552. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654314561338
  • Jausovec, N., & Jausovec, K. (2012). Working memory training: Improving intelligence-changing brain activity. Brain & Cognition, 79(2), 96–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.007
  • Karbach, J., Könen, T., & Spengler, M. (2017). Who benefits the most? Individual differences in the transfer of executive control training across the lifespan. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 1(4), 394–405. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-017-0054-z
  • Karbach, J., & Kray, J. (2009). How useful is executive control training? Age differences in near and far transfer of task-switching training. Developmental Science, 12(6), 978–990. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00846.x
  • Karbach, J., & Schubert, T. (2013). Training-induced cognitive and neural plasticity. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 48. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00048
  • Karbach, J., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2015). Adaptive working memory training benefits reading, but not mathematics in middle childhood. Child Neuropsychology, 21(3), 285–301. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2014.899336
  • Kattner, F., Samaan, L., & Schubert, T. (2019). Cross-modal transfer after auditory task-switching training. Memory & Cognition, 1–18. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-019-00911-x
  • Kiesel, A., Steinhauser, M., Wendt, M., Falkenstein, M., Jost, K., Philipp, A. M., & Koch, I. (2010). Control and interference in task switching—A review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(5), 849–874. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0019842
  • Klingberg, T., Fernell, E., Olesen, P. J., Johnson, M., Gustafsson, P., Dahlström, K., & Westerberg, H. (2005). Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD-a randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(2), 177–186. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010
  • Koch, I., Gade, M., Schuch, S., & Philipp, A. M. (2010). The role of inhibition in task switching: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 17(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.3758/PBR.17.1.1
  • Kray, J., & Fehér, B. (2017). Age differences in the transfer and maintenance of practice-induced improvements in task switching: The impact of working-memory and inhibition demands. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 410. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00410
  • Kray, J., Karbach, J., Haenig, S., & Freitag, C. (2012). Can task-switching training enhance executive control functioning in children with attention deficit/-hyperactivity disorder? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 5, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2011.00180
  • Lawson, G. M., Duda, J. T., Avants, B. B., Wu, J., & Farah, M. J. (2013). Associations between children’s socioeconomic status and prefrontal cortical thickness. Developmental Science, 16(5), 641–652. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12096
  • Lawson, G. M., Hook, C. J., & Farah, M. J. (2018). A meta-analysis of the relationship between socioeconomic status and executive function performance among children. Developmental Science, 21(2), e12529. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12529
  • Li, J., Zhao, Y., Zhou, S., Pu, Y., He, H., & Zhao, M. (2020). Set-shifting ability is specifically linked to high-school science and math achievement in Chinese adolescents. PsyCh Journal, 9(3), 327–338. https://doi.org/10.1002/pchj.328
  • Lillard, A., & Else-Quest, N. (2006). The early years: Evaluating montessori education. Science, 313(5795), 1893–1894. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1132362
  • Liu, Q., Zhu, X., Ziegler, A., & Shi, J. (2015). The effects of inhibitory control training for preschoolers on reasoning ability and neural activity. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 14200. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep14200
  • Lugo-Gil, J., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2008). Family resources and parenting quality: Links to children’s cognitive development across the first 3 years. Child Development, 79(4), 1065–1085. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01176.x
  • Lyons, K. E., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Monitoring, metacognition, and executive function: Elucidating the role of self-reflection in the development of self-regulation. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 40, 379–412. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-386491-8.00010-4
  • Mahncke, H. W., Connor, B. B., Appelman, J., Ahsanuddin, O. N., Hardy, J. L., Wood, R. A., Joyce, N. M., Boniske, T., Atkins, S. M., & Merzenich, M. M. (2006). Memory enhancement in healthy older adults using a brain plasticity-based training program: A randomized, controlled study. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 103(33), 12523–12528. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0605194103
  • Marcovitch, S., Jacques, S., Boseovski, J. J., & Zelazo, P. D. (2008). Self-reflection and the cognitive control of behavior: Implications for learning. Mind, Brain, and Education, 2(3), 136–141. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-228X.2008.00044.x
  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2016). There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: A reply to Au et al. (2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 324–330. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0862-z
  • Miller, M. R., Müller, U., Giesbrecht, G. F., Carpendale, J. I., & Kerns, K. A. (2013). The contribution of executive function and social understanding to preschoolers’ letter and math skills. Cognitive Development, 28(4), 331–349. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2012.10.005
  • Minear, M., & Shah, P. (2008). Training and transfer effects in task switching. Memory & Cognition, 36(8), 1470–1483. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.336.8.1470
  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1999.0734
  • Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(03)00028-7
  • Nesbitt, K. T., Baker-Ward, L., & Willoughby, M. T. (2013). Executive function mediates socio-economic and racial differences in early academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(4), 774–783. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.07.005
  • Noble, K. G., Houston, S. M., Brito, N. H., Bartsch, H., Kan, E., Kuperman, J. M., Akshoomoff, N., Amaral, D. G., Bloss, C. S., Libiger, O., & Schork, N. J. (2015). Family income, parental education and brain structure in children and adolescents. Nature Neuroscience, 18(5), 773–778. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3983
  • O’Connor, T. G., Rutter, M., Beckett, C., Keaveney, L., & Kreppner, J. M., & English and Romanian Adoptees Study Team. (2000). The effects of global severe privation on cognitive competence: Extension and longitudinal follow-up. Child Development, 71(2), 376–390. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8624.00151
  • Olesen, P. J., Westerberg, H., & Klingberg, T. (2004). Increased prefrontal and parietal activity after training of working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 7(1), 75–79. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1165
  • Peng, P., Namkung, J., Barnes, M., & Sun, C. (2016). A meta-analysis of mathematics and working memory: Moderating effects of working memory domain, type of mathematics skill, and sample characteristics. Journal of Educational Psychology, 108(4), 455–473. https://doi.org/10.1037/edu0000079
  • Peng, P., Wang, C., & Namkung, J. (2018). Understanding the cognition related to mathematics difficulties: A meta-analysis on the cognitive deficit profiles and the bottleneck theory. Review of Educational Research, 88(3), 434–476. https://doi.org/10.3102/0034654317753350
  • Pereg, M., Shahar, N., & Meiran, N. (2013). Task switching effects are mediated by working-memory management. Intelligence, 41(5), 467–478. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2013.06.009
  • Philipp, A. M., Kalinich, C., Koch, I., & Schubotz, R. I. (2008). Mixing costs and switch costs when switching stimulus dimensions in serial predictions. Psychological Research, 72(4), 406–414. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-008-0150-x
  • Poon, K., & Ho, C. S. H. (2014). Contrasting deficits on executive functions in Chinese delinquent adolescents with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder symptoms and/or reading disability. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 35(11), 3046–3056. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ridd.2014.07.046
  • Powers, K. L., Brooks, P. J., Aldrich, N. J., Palladino, M. A., & Alfieri, L. (2013). Effects of video-game play on information processing: A meta-analytic investigation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 20(6), 1055–1079. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-013-0418-z
  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Manual for Raven's progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 3: The Standard Progressive Matrices. The Psychological Corporation.
  • Rogers, D. R., & Monsell, S. (1995). Costs of predictable switch between simple cognitive tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(2), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.124.2.207
  • Rubin, O., & Meiran, N. (2005). On the origins of the task mixing cost in the cuing task-switching paradigm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31(6), 1477–1491. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.31.6.1477
  • Sabah, K., Dolk, T., Meiran, N., & Dreisbach, G. (2018). When less is more: Costs and benefits of varied vs. fixed content and structure in short-term task switching training. Psychological Research, 83(7), 1531–1542. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1006-7.
  • Samavatyan, H., & Leth-Steensen, C. (2009). The time course of task switching: A speed—accuracy trade-off analysis. Memory & Cognition, 37(7), 1051–1058. https://doi.org/10.3758/MC.37.7.1051
  • Sektnan, M., McClelland, M. M., Acock, A., & Morrison, F. J. (2010). Relations between early family risk, child’en's behavioral regulation, and academic achievement. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25(4), 464–479. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2010.02.005
  • Shawn Green, C., Bavelier, D., Kramer, A. F., Vinogradov, S., Ansorge, U., & Ball, K.K., ... & Witt, C. (2019). Improving methodological standards in behavioral interventions for cognitive enhancement. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 3(1), 2–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-018-0115-y.
  • Soveri, A., Waris, O., & Laine, M. (2013). Set shifting training with categorization tasks. PlosOne, 8(12), e81693. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0081693
  • Strobach, T., Liepelt, R., Schubert, T., & Kiesel, A. (2012). Task switching: Effects of practice on switch and mixing costs. Psychological Research, 76(1), 74–83. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-011-0323-x
  • Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies in interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0054651
  • Titz, C., & Karbach, J. (2014). Working memory and executive functions: Effects of training on academic achievement. Psychological Research, 78(6), 852–868. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0537-1
  • Traverso, L., Viterbori, P., & Usai, M. C. (2019). Effectiveness of an executive function training in Italian preschool educational services and far transfer effects to pre-academic skills. Frontier in Psychology, 10, 2053. doi:doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02053
  • Viterbori, P., Usai, M. C., Traverso, L., & De Franchis, V. (2015). How preschool executive functioning predicts several aspects of math achievement in Grades 1 and 3: A longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 140, 38–55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2015.06.014
  • von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(1), 36–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.02.002
  • Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S. H., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., … Sung, M. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies: The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(3), 621–633. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2013.04.002
  • Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Tominey, S. L., & Acock, A. C. (2011). The influence of demographic risk factors on child’en's behavioral regulation in prekindergarten and kindergarten. Early Education & Development, 22(3), 461–488. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2011.536132
  • Wass, S. V., Scerif, G., & Johnson, M. H. (2012). Training attentional control and working memo–y - Is younger, better? Developmental Review, 32(4), 360–387. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2012.07.001
  • Wechsler, D. (1981). WAIS-R Manual: The Wechsler Adult intelligence scale-Revised. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich [for] The Psychological Corporation.
  • Weissheimer, J., Fuji, R. C., & de Souza, J. G. M. (2019). The effects of cognitive training on executive functions and reading in typically developing children with varied socioeconomic status in Brazil. Ilha Desterro, 72(3), 85–100. https://doi.org/10.5007/2175-8026.2019v72n3p85
  • Welsh, J. A., Nix, R. L., Blair, C., Bierman, K. L., & Nelson, K. E. (2010). The development of cognitive skills and gains in academic school readiness for children from low-income families. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102(1), 43–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016738
  • White, H. A., & Shah, P. (2006). Training attention-switching ability in adults with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10(1), 44–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1087054705286063
  • Yeniad, N., Malda, M., Mesman, J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., & Pieper, S. (2013). Shifting ability predicts math and reading performance in children: A meta-analytical study. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.10.004
  • Zelazo, P. D., Anderson, J. E., Richler, J., Wallner-Allen, K., Beaumont, J. L., & Weintraub, S. (2013). II. NIH Toolbox cognition Battery (CB): measuring executive function and attention. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 78(4), 16–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/mono.12032
  • Zhang, Q., Wang, C., Zhao, Q., Yang, L., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2019). The malleability of executive function in early childhood: Effects of schooling and targeted training. Developmental Science, 22(2), e12748. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12748
  • Zhao, X., Wang, H., & Maes, J. H. R. (2018). Training and transfer effects of extensive task-switching training in students. Psychological Research, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1059-7
  • Zimmerman, B. J. (2008). Investigating self-regulation and motivation: Historical background, methodological developments, and future prospects. American Educational Research Journal, 45(1), 166–183. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831207312909
  • Zinke, K., Einert, M., Pfennig, L., & Kliegel, M. (2012). Plasticity of executive control through task switching training in adolescents. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 41. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00041
  • Zinke, K., Zeintl, M., Rose, N. S., Putzmann, J., Pydde, A., & Kliegel, M. (2014). Working memory training and transfer in older adults: Effects of age, baseline performance, and training gains. Developmental Psychology, 50(1), 304–315. doi:10.1037/a0032982