863
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

Detecting emotion faces in a Posner’s spatial cueing task: the adaptive value of surprise

ORCID Icon, , &
Pages 38-48 | Received 04 Jun 2020, Accepted 07 Dec 2020, Published online: 22 Dec 2020

References

  • Agnew, N., & Agnew, M. (1963). Drive level effects on tasks of narrow and broad attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 15(1), 58–62. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470216308416552
  • Akhtar, N., & Enns, J. T. (1989). Relations between convert orienting and filtering in the development of visual attention. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48(2), 315–334. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-0965(89)90008-8
  • Bannerman, R. L., Milders, M., de Gelder, B., & Sahraie, A. (2008). Orienting to threat: Faster localization of fearful facial expressions and body postures revealed by saccadic eye movements. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1662), 1635–1641. /https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2008.1744
  • Calvo, M. G., Avero, P., & Lundqvist, D. (2006). Facilitated detection of angry faces: Initial orienting and processing efficiency. Cognition and Emotion, 20(6), 785–811. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500465224
  • Cohen-Gilbert, J. E., & Thomas, K. M. (2013). Inhibitory control during emotional distraction across adolescence and early adulthood. Child Development, 84(6), 1954–1966. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12085
  • Eastwood, J. D., Smilek, D., & Merikle, P. M. (2001). Differential attentional guidance by unattended faces expressing positive and negative emotion. Perception & Psychophysics, 63(6), 1004–1013. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03194519
  • Esposito, F. L., & Supèr, H. (2018). Vergence responses to face stimuli in young children. NeuroReport, 29(3), 219–223. https://doi.org/10.1097/WNR.0000000000000963
  • Estéphan, A., Fiset, D., Saumure, C., Plouffe-Demers, M.-P., Zhang, Y., Sun, D., & Blais, C. (2018). Time course of cultural differences in spatial frequency use for face identification. Scientific Reports, 8(1), 1816. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-19971-1
  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A.-G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03193146
  • Fox, E., Lester, R., Russo, R., Bowles, R. J., Pichler, A., & Dutton, K. (2000). Facial expressions of emotion: Are Angry faces detected more efficiently? Cognition and Emotion, 14(1), 61–92. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300378996
  • Goffaux, V., Hault, B., Michel, C., Vuong, Q. C., & Rossion, B. (2005). The respective role of low and high spatial frequencies in supporting configural and featural processing of faces. Perception, 34(1), 77–86. https://doi.org/10.1068/p5370
  • Gordillo, F., Mestas, L., Pérez, M. A., & Arana, J. M. (2018). Role of surprise in the discrimination of the facial expression of fear: Part of a continuum? The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 21, E3. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2018.5
  • Gordillo, F., Mestas, L., Pérez, M. A., Escotto, E. A., & Arana, J. M. (2019). The priming effect of a facial expression of surprise on the discrimination of a facial expression of fear. Current Psychology. Current Psychology, 38, 1613–1621. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-017-9719-0
  • Hu, K., Bauer, A., Padmala, S., & Pessoa, L. (2012). Threat of bodily harm has opposing effects on cognition. Emotion, 12(1), 28–32. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024345
  • Hunt, A. R., & Kingstone, A. (2003). Inhibition of return: Dissociating attentional and oculomotor components. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29(5), 1068–1074. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.29.5.1068
  • Jack, R. E., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. G. (2014). Dynamic facial expressions of emotion transmit an evolving hierarchy of signals over time. Current Biology, 24(2), 187–192. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.11.064
  • Jack, R. E., Sun, W., Delis, I., Garrod, O. G. B., & Schyns, P. G. (2016). Four not six: Revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145(6), 708–730. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000162
  • Kuefner, D., Cassia, V. M., Vescovo, E., & Picozzi, M. (2010). Natural experience acquired in adulthood enhances holistic processing of other-age faces. Visual Cognition, 18(1), 11–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/13506280802396507
  • Leppänen, J. M., & Nelson, C. A. (2009). Tuning the developing brain to social signals of emotions. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 37–47. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2554
  • Mulckhuyse, M. (2018). The influence of emotional stimuli on the oculomotor system: A review of the literature. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(3), 411–425. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-018-0590-8
  • Öhman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.130.3.466
  • Parkinson, J., Garfinkel, S., Critchley, H., Dienes, Z., & Seth, A. K. (2017). Don’t make me angry, you wouldn’t like me when I’m angry: Volitional choices to act or inhibit are modulated by subliminal perception of emotional faces. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 17(2), 252–268. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-016-0477-5
  • Pinkham, A. E., Griffin, M., Baron, R., Sasson, N. J., & Gur, R. C. (2010). The face in the crowd effect: Anger superiority when using real faces and multiple identities. Emotion, 10(1), 141–146. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017387
  • Posner, M. I., Klein, R., Summers, J., & Buggie, S. (1973). On the selection of signals. Memory & Cognition, 1(1), 2–12. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03198062
  • Posner, M. I., & Peterson, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
  • Posner, M. I., Snyder, C. R. R., & Davidson, B. J. (1980). Attention and the detection of signals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 109(2), 160–174. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.109.2.160
  • Robinson, O. J., Vytal, K., Cornwell, B. R., & Grillon, C. (2013). The impact of anxiety upon cognition: Perspectives from human threat of shock studies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00203
  • Sawada, R., & Sato, W. (2015). Emotional attention capture by facial expressions. Scientific Reports, 5(1), 14042. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep14042
  • Sawada, R., Sato, W., Kochiyama, T., Uono, S., Kubota, Y., Yoshimura, S., & Toichi, M. (2014). Sex differences in the rapid detection of emotional facial expressions. PLOS ONE, 9(4), e94747. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0094747
  • Simpson, J. R., Ongur, D., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E., Ollinger, J. M., Snyder, A., Snyder, A. Z., Gusnard, D. A., & Raichle, M. E. (2000). The emotional modulation of cognitive processing: An fMRI study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 12(Suppl 2), 157–170. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892900564019
  • Stein, T., Seymour, K., Hebart, M. N., & Sterzer, P. (2014). Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies. Psychological Science, 25(2), 566–574. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613512509
  • Tecce, J. T., & Happ, S. J. (1964). Effects of shock-arousal on a card-sorting test of color-word interference. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19(3), 905–906. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1964.19.3.905
  • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., Marcus, D. J., Westerlund, A., Casey, B. J., & Nelson, C. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006
  • Townsend, J. T., & Ashby, F. G. (1983). Stochastic modeling of elementary psychological processes. Cambridge University Press.
  • Veling, H., Aarts, H., & Stroebe, W. (2011). Fear signals inhibit impulsive behavior toward rewarding food objects. Appetite, 56(3), 643–648. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2011.02.018
  • Verbruggen, F., & Houwer, J. D. (2007). Do emotional stimuli interfere with response inhibition? Evidence from the stop signal paradigm. Cognition and Emotion, 21(2), 391–403. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930600625081

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.