353
Views
1
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Articles

The near- and far-transfer effects of cognitive training on attentional networks in women and men

, , , &
Pages 453-469 | Received 08 Sep 2020, Accepted 08 Apr 2021, Published online: 18 Apr 2021

References

  • Baniqued, P., Kranz, M., Voss, M., Lee, H., Cosman, J., Severson, J., & Kramer, A. F. (2014). Cognitive training with casual video games: Points to consider. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 1010. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00234
  • Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). When and where do we apply what we learn?: A taxonomy for far transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 128(4), 612–637. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-2909.128.4.612
  • Bayliss, A. P., di Pellegrino, G., & Tipper, S. P. (2005). Sex differences in eye gaze and symbolic cueing of attention. Quarterly Journal Experimental Psychology. A, Human Experimental Psychology, 58(4), 631–650. https://doi.org/10.1080/02724980443000124
  • Boot, W. R., Kramer, A. F., Simons, D. J., Fabiani, M., & Gratton, G. (2008). The effects of video game playing on attention, memory, and executive control. Acta Psychologica, 129(3), 387–398. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2008.09.005
  • Bunting, M., Cowan, N., & Saults, J. S. (2006). How does running memory span work? Quarterly Journal Experimental Psychology, 59(10), 1691–1700. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210600848402
  • Callejas, A., Lupiáñez, J., & Tudela, P. (2004). The three attentional networks: On their independence and interactions. Brain and Cognition, 54(3), 225–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2004.02.012
  • Carlson, S., Martinkauppi, S., Rämä, P., Salli, E., Korvenoja, K., & Aronen, H. J. (1998). Distribution of cortical activation during visuospatial n-back tasks as revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging. Cerebral Cortex, 8(8), 743–752. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/8.8.743
  • Chooi, W. T., & Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence, 40(6), 531–542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2012.07.004
  • Clayson, P. E., Clawson, A., & Larson, M. J. (2011). Sex differences in electrophysiological indices of conflict monitoring. Biological Psychology, 87(2), 282–289. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2011.03.011
  • Dahlin, E., Neely, A. S., Larsson, A., Bäckman, L., & Nyberg, L. (2008). Transfer of learning after updating training mediated by the striatum. Science, 320(5882), 1510–1512. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1155466
  • Dickie, E. W., & Armony, J. L. (2008). Amygdala responses to unattended fearful faces: Interaction between sex and trait anxiety. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 162(1), 51–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.08.002
  • Dougherty, M. R., Hamovitz, T., & Tidwell, J. W. (2016). Reevaluating the effectiveness of n-back training on transfer through the Bayesian lens: Support for the null. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 306–316. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0865-9
  • Duda, B. M., & Sweet, L. H. (2020). Functional brain changes associated with cognitive training in healthy older adults: A preliminary ALE meta-analysis. Brain Imaging and Behavior, 14(4), 1247–1262. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11682-019-00080-0
  • Fan, J., Flombaum, J. I., McCandliss, B. D., Thomas, K. M., & Posner, M. I. (2003). Cognitive and brain consequences of conflict. Neuroimage, 18(1), 42–57. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1319
  • Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A., & Posner, M. I. (2002). Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(3), 340–347. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892902317361886
  • Fernandez-Duque, D., & Posner, M. I. (2001). Brain imaging of attentional networks in normal and pathological states. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23(1), 74–93. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.1.74.1217
  • Friedman, N. P., Miyake, A., Corley, R. P., Young, S. E., DeFries, J. C., & Hewitt, J. K. (2006). Not all executive functions are related to intelligence. Psychological Science, 17(2), 172–179. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01681.x
  • Gathercole, S. E., Dunning, D. L., Holmes, J., & Norris, D. (2019). “Working memory training involves learning new skills”: corrigendum. Journal of Memory and Language, 106, 203. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2019.02.004
  • Green, C. S., Strobach, T., & Schubert, T. (2014). On methodological standards in training and transfer experiments. Psychological Research, 78(6), 756–772. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-013-0535-3
  • Huang, A. S., Klein, D. N., & Leung, H.-C. (2016). Load-related brain activation predicts spatial working memory performance in youth aged 9–12 and is associated with executive function at earlier ages. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.10.007
  • Hulme, C., & Melby-Lervåg, M. (2012). Current evidence does not support the claims made for CogMed working memory training. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(3), 197–200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.06.006
  • Iordan, A. D., Cooke, K. A., Moored, K. D., Katz, B., Buschkuehl, M., Jaeggi, S. M., Polk, T. A., Peltier, S. J., Jonides, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2020). Neural correlates of working memory training: Evidence for plasticity in older adults. NeuroImage, 217, 116887. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116887
  • Jha, A. P., Krompinger, J., & Baime, M. J. (2007). Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7(2), 109–119. https://doi.org/10.3758/CABN.7.2.109
  • Karbach, J., & Verhaeghen, P. (2014). Making working memory work: A meta-analysis of executive-control and working memory training in older adults. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2027–2037. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614548725
  • Kirchner, W. K. (1958). Age differences in short-term retention of rapidly changing information. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 55(4), 352–358. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043688
  • Klingberg, T. (2010). Training and plasticity of working memory. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(7), 317–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.05.002
  • Könen, T., Strobach, T., & Karbach, J. (2016). Working memory. In T. Strobach, & J. Karbach (Eds.), Cognitive training: An overview of features and applications (pp. 59–68). Springer International Publishing. doi:10.1007/978-3-319-42662-4_6.
  • Kundu, B., Sutterer, D. W., Emrich, S. M., & Postle, B. R. (2013). Strengthened effective connectivity underlies transfer of working memory training to tests of short-term memory and attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(20), 8705–8715. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5565-12.2013
  • Liu, G., Hu, P. P., Fan, J., & Wang, K. (2013). Gender differences associated with orienting attentional networks in healthy subjects. Chinese Medical Journal, 126(12), 2308–2312.
  • Loosli, S. V., Buschkuehl, M., Perrig, W. J., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2012). Working memory training improves reading processes in typically developing children. Child Neuropsychology, 18(1), 62–78. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2011.575772
  • Lorch Jr, R. F., & Myers, J. L. (1990). Regression analyses of repeated measures data in cognitive research. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 16(1), 149–157. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.16.1.149
  • Luna, F., Marino, J., Macbeth, G., & Torres, G. (2016). Are there sex differences in attentional networks? A systematic review of the phasic alerting, vigilance, orienting and executive control. Psychologia, 10(2), 63–71. https://doi.org/10.21500/19002386.2283
  • Mayr, U., & Kliegl, R. (2000). Task-set switching and long-term memory retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26(5), 1124–1140. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.26.5.1124
  • Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270–291. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228
  • Merritt, P., Hirshman, E., Wharton, W., Stangl, B., Devlin, J., & Lenz, A. (2007). Evidence for gender differences in visual selective attention. Personality and Individual Differences, 43(3), 597–609. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2007.01.016
  • Miró, E., Martínez, M. P., Sánchez, A. I., Prados, G., & Lupiáñez, J. (2015). Men and women with fibromyalgia: Relation between attentional function and clinical symptoms. British Journal of Health Psychology, 20(3), 632–647. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjhp.12128
  • Miyake, A., & Shah, P. (1999). Models of working memory: Mechanisms of active maintenance and executive control. Cambridge University Press.
  • Morrison, A. B., & Chein, J. M. (2012). The controversy over cogmed. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 1(3), 208–210. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jarmac.2012.07.005
  • Nguyen, L., Murphy, K., & Andrews, G. (2019). Immediate and long-term efficacy of executive functions cognitive training in older adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 145(7), 698–733. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000196.supp
  • Noack, H., Lövdén, M., Schmiedek, F., & Lindenberger, U. (2009). Cognitive plasticity in adulthood and old age: Gauging the generality of cognitive intervention effects. Restorative Neurology and Neuroscience, 27(5), 435–453. https://doi.org/10.3233/RNN-2009-0496
  • Novick, J. M., Bunting, M. F., Dougherty, M. R., & Engle, R. W. (2019). Cognitive and working memory training: Perspectives from psychology, neuroscience and human development. Oxford University Press.
  • Oberauer, K. (2015). Training of intelligence: A question of intelligent training. https://featuredcontent.psychonomic.org/training-of-intelligence-a-question-of-intelligent-training/.
  • Oldrati, V., Corti, C., Poggi, G., Borgatti, R., Urgesi, C., & Bardoni, A. (2020). Effectiveness of computerized cognitive training programs (CCTP) with game-like features in children with or without neuropsychological disorders: A meta-analytic investigation. Neuropsychology Review, 30(1), 126–141. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-020-09429-5
  • Owen, A. M., Hampshire, A., Grahn, J. A., Stenton, R., Dajani, S., Burns, A. S., Howard, R. J., & Ballard, C. G. (2010). Putting brain training to the test. Nature., 465(7299), 775–778. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature09042
  • Ozeki, M., & Yokosawa, K. (2003). The properties of visual short-term memory on change detection task. The Japanese Journal of Psychology, 73(6), 464–471. https://doi.org/10.4992/jjpsy.73.464
  • Pergher, V., Shalchy, M. A., Pahor, A., Van Hulle, M. M., Jaeggi, S. M., & Seitz, A. R. (2019). Divergent research methods limit understanding of working memory training. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 4(1), 100–120. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41465-019-00134-7
  • Persson, J., & Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2008). Gaining control: Training executive function and far transfer of the ability to resolve interference. Psychological Science, 19(9), 881–888. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02172.x
  • Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system of the human brain: 20 years after. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 35(1), 73–89. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150525
  • Pollack, I., Johnson, L. B., & Knaff, P. R. (1959). Running memory span. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57(3), 137–146. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0046137
  • Posner, M. I. (1994). Attention: The mechanisms of consciousness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 91(16), 7398–7403. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.91.16.7398
  • Posner, M. I., & Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13(1), 25–42. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325
  • Posner, M. I., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 1–23. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085516
  • Pugin, F., Metz, A. J., Stauffer, M., Wolf, M., Jenni, O. G., & Huber, R. (2014). Working memory training shows immediate and long-term effects on cognitive performance in children. F1000Research, 3, 82. https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.3665.2
  • Rawle, C. J., Miall, R. C., & Praamstra, P. (2012). Frontoparietal theta activity supports behavioral decisions in movement-target selection. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 138. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00138
  • Redick, T. S. (2015). Working memory training and interpreting interactions in intelligence interventions. Intelligence, 50, 14–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2015.01.014
  • Redick, T. S., Broadway, J. M., Meier, M. E., Kuriakose, P. S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28(3), 164–171. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000123
  • Redick, T. S., Shipstead, Z., Wiemers, E. A., Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2015). What's working in working memory training? An educational perspective. Educational Psychology Review, 27(4), 617–633. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-015-9314-6
  • Rensink, R. A. (1996). The attentional capacity of visual search under flicker conditions. Perception, 25(1_suppl), 125–125. https://doi.org/10.1068/v96l0105
  • Roca, J., García-Fernández, P., Castro, C., & Lupiáñez, J. (2018). The moderating effects of vigilance on other components of attentional functioning. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 308, 151–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2018.07.019
  • Rosenthal, R., & Rosnow, R. L. (2008). Essentials of behavioral research: Methods and data analysis (3rd ed.). McGraw-Hill.
  • Rothbart, M. K., Posner, M. I., & Rosicky, J. (1994). Orienting in normal and pathological development. Development and Psychopathology, 6(4), 635–652. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579400004715
  • Rueda, M. R., Checa, P., & Cómbita, L. M. (2012). Enhanced efficiency of the executive attention network after training in preschool children: Immediate changes and effects after two months. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2(Suppl 1), S192–S204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2011.09.004
  • Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L., & Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic influences on the development of executive attention. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102(41), 14931–14936. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0506897102
  • Sala, G., Aksayli, N. D., Tatlidil, K. S., Gondo, Y., & Gobet, F. (2019). Working memory training does not enhance older adults’ cognitive skills: A comprehensive meta-analysis. Intelligence, 77, 101386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.intell.2019.101386
  • Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017). Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 515–520. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417712760
  • Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2020). Working memory training in typically developing children: A multilevel meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27 (3), 423–434. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-019-01681-y
  • Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138–166. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2015.1036274
  • Scionti, N., Cavallero, M., Zogmaister, C., & Marzocchi, G. M. (2020). “Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis”: corrigendum. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2812. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00410
  • Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628–654. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027473
  • Smoleń, T., Jastrzebski, J., Estrada, E., & Chuderski, A. (2018). Most evidence for the compensation account of cognitive training is unreliable. Memory & Cognition, 46(8), 1315–1330. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-018-0839-z
  • Soveri, A., Antfolk, J., Karlsson, L., Salo, B., & Laine, M. (2017). Working memory training revisited: A multi-level meta-analysis of n-back training studies. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 24(4), 1077–1096. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1217-0
  • Strobach, T., & Karbach, J. (2016). Cognitive training. An overview of features and applications. Springer International Publishing.
  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2013). Using. (6th ed.). Pearson Education.
  • Tang, Y. Y., Ma, Y., Wang, J., Fan, Y., Feng, S., Lu, Q., Yu, Q., Sui, D., Rothbart, M. K., Fan, M., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(43), 17152–17156. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0707678104
  • Tang, Y. Y., & Posner, M. I. (2009). Attention training and attention state training. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(5), 222–227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.009
  • Teixeira-Santos, A. C., Moreira, C. S., Magalhães, R., Magalhães, C., Pereira, D. R., Leite, J., Carvalho, S., & Sampaio, A. (2019). Reviewing working memory training gains in healthy older adults: A meta-analytic review of transfer for cognitive outcomes. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 103, 163–177. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.05.009
  • Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., Garel, K. L., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Reynolds, G. O., Winter, R., Chang, P., Pollard, K., Lala, N., Alvarez, G. A., Gabrieli, J. D. E., & de Beeck, H. P. O. p. (2013). Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLoS One, 8(5), e63614. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0063614
  • Treisman, A. M., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12(1), 97–136. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5
  • van Balkom, T. D., van den Heuvel, O. A., Berendse, H. W., van der Werf, Y. D., & Vriend, C. (2020). The effects of cognitive training on brain network activity and connectivity in aging and neurodegenerative diseases: A systematic review. Neuropsychology Review, 30(2), 267–286. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-020-09440-w
  • von Bastian, C. C., Langer, N., Jäncke, L., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Memory & Cognition, 41(4), 611–624. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-012-0280-7
  • von Bastian, C. C., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Distinct transfer effects of training different facets of working memory capacity. Journal of Memory and Language, 69(1), 36–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2013.02.002
  • Williams, L. M., Barton, M. J., Kemp, A. H., Liddell, B. J., Peduto, A., Gordon, E., & Bryant, R. A. (2005). Distinct amygdala–autonomic arousal profiles in response to fear signals in healthy males and females. Neuroimage, 28(3), 618–626. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.06.035

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.