149
Views
0
CrossRef citations to date
0
Altmetric
Review Article

(Non)automaticity of ritualized behavior

ORCID Icon
Pages 384-400 | Received 07 Mar 2020, Accepted 29 Apr 2022, Published online: 17 Jun 2022

References

  • Anastasi, M. W., & Newberg, A. B. (2008). A preliminary study of the acute effects of religious ritual on anxiety. Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 14(2), 163–165. https://doi.org/10.1089/acm.2007.0675
  • Anderson, J. R. (1992). Automaticity and the ACT* theory. The American Journal of Psychology, 105(2), 165–180. https://doi.org/10.2307/1423026
  • Ayaz, H., Shewokis, P. A., Bunce, S., Izzetoglu, K., Willems, B., & Onaral, B. (2012). Optical brain monitoring for operator training and mental workload assessment. Neuroimage, 59(1), 36–47. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.06.023
  • Baddeley, A. (2012). Working memory: Theories, models, and controversies. Annual Review of Psychology, 63(1), 1–29. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422
  • Bayot, M., Dujardin, K., Tard, C., Defebvre, L., Bonnet, C. T., Allart, E., & Delval, A. (2018). The interaction between cognition and motor control: A theoretical framework for dual-task interference effects on posture, gait initiation, gait and turning. Neurophysiologie Clinique, 48(6), 361–375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neucli.2018.10.003
  • Boiche, J., Marchant, G., Nicaise, V., & Bison, A. (2016). Development of the Generic Multifaceted Automaticity Scale (GMAS) and preliminary validation for physical activity. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 25, 60–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychsport.2016.03.003
  • Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595–613. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06009332
  • Brooks, A. W., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Gino, F., Galinsky, A. D., Norton, M. I., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2016). Don’t stop believing: Rituals improve performance by decreasing anxiety. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137(1), 71–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.07.004
  • Clark, D. J. (2015). Automaticity of walking: Functional significance, mechanisms, measurement and rehabilitation strategies. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 246. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00246
  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 135–168. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750
  • Eilam, D. (2006). Ritualized behavior in animals and humans: Time, space, and attention. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 616–617. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0600937X
  • Eilam, D., Zor, R., Szechtman, H., & Hermesh, H. (2006). Rituals, stereotypy and compulsive behavior in animals and humans. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 30(4), 456–471. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.08.003
  • Ersche, K. D., Lim, T. V., Ward, L. H., Robbins, T. W., & Stochl, J. (2017). Creature of Habit: a self-report measure of habitual routines and automatic tendencies in everyday life. Personality and Individual Differences, 116, 73–85. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2017.04.024
  • Friedman, N. P., & Miyake, A. (2017). Unity and diversity of executive functions: Individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex, 86(1), 186–204. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023
  • Gage, W. H., Sleik, R. J., Polych, M. A., McKenzie, N. C., & Brown, L. A. (2003). The allocation of attention during locomotion is altered by anxiety. Experimental Brain Research, 150(3), 385–394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-003-1468-7
  • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415(6873), 755. https://doi.org/10.1038/415755a
  • Haith, A. M., & Krakauer, J. W. (2018). The multiple effects of practice: Skill, habit and reduced cognitive load. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 20(1), 196–201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.015
  • Hélie, S., & Cousineau, D. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of automaticity: Behavioral and brain signatures. Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 25–43.
  • Hobson, N. M., Schroeder, J., Risen, J. L., Xygalatas, D., & Inzlicht, M. (2018). The Psychology of rituals: An Integrative review and process-based framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(3), 260–284. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868317734944
  • Huppert, J. D., & Cahill, S. P. (2006). What is the relevance of boyer & lienard's model for psychosocial treatments? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 620–621. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06009411
  • Ju, Y. J., & Lien, Y. W. (2016). Better control with less effort: The advantage of using focused-breathing strategy over focused-distraction strategy on thought suppression. Consciousness and Cognition, 40(1), 9–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.12.002
  • Karl, J. A., & Fischer, R. (2018). Rituals, repetitiveness and cognitive load. Human Nature, 29(4), 418–441. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-018-9325-3
  • Krátký, J., Lang, M., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D., & Xygalatas, D. (2016). Anxiety and ritualization: Can attention discriminate compulsion from routine? Communicative and Integrative Biology, 9(3), e117479. https://doi.org/10.1080/19420889.2016.1174799
  • LaBerge, D., & Samuels, S. J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6(2), 293–323. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(74)90015-2
  • Lang, M., Krátký, J., Shaver, J. H., Jerotijević, D., & Xygalatas, D. (2015). Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior. Current Biology, 25(14), 1892–1897. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.049
  • Lang, M., Krátký, J., & Xygalatas, D. (2020). The role of ritual behaviour in anxiety reduction: An investigation of marathi religious practices in Mauritius. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 375(1805), 20190431. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0431
  • Logan, G. D. (1988). Toward an instance theory of automatization. Psychological Review, 95(4), 492–527. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.95.4.492
  • Lopez, A., Caffò, A. O., Tinella, L., & Bosco, A. (2021). The four factors of Mind Wandering Questionnaire: Content, construct, and clinical validity. Assessment, https://doi.org/10.1177/10731911211058688
  • MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of reseach on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163–203. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.109.2.163
  • Matsuzaka, Y., Picard, N., & Strick, P. L. (2007). Skill representation in the primary motor cortex after long-term practice. Journal of Neurophysiology, 97(2), 1819–1832. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00784.2006
  • Matthews, G. (2021). Stress states, personality and cognitive functioning: A review of research with the Dundee Stress State questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 169, 110083. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110083
  • Matthews, G., Campbell, S. E., Falconer, S., Joyner, L. A., Huggins, J., Gilliland, K., Grier, R., & Warm, J. S. (2002). Fundamental dimensions of subjective state in performance settings: Task engagement, distress, and worry. Emotion, 2(4), 315–340. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.2.4.315
  • Matthews, G., Joyner, L., Gilliland, K., Campbell, S., Falconer, S., & Huggins, J. (1999). Validation of a comprehensive stress state questionnaire: Towards a state ‘Big Three’?. In I. Mervielde, I. J. Dreary, F. DeFruyt, & F. Ostendorf (Eds.), Personality Psychology in Europe (Vol. 7, pp. 335–350). Tilburg, the Netherlands: Tilburg University Press.
  • McCauley, R. N. (2006). How far will an account of ritualized behavior go in explaining cultural rituals?. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 623–624. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06009447
  • Moors, A., & De Houwer, J. (2006). Automaticity: A theoretical and conceptual analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 297–326. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.297
  • Mrazek, M. D., Phillips, D. T., Franklin, M. S., Broadway, J. M., & Schooler, J. W. (2013). Young and restless: Validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 560. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00560
  • Nielbo, K. L., Schjoedt, U., & Sørensen, J. (2013). Hierarchical organization of segmentation in non-functional action sequences. Journal for the Cognitive Science of Religion, 1(1), 71–97. https://doi.org/10.1558/jcsr.v1i1.71
  • Nielbo, K. L., & Sørensen, J. (2011). Spontaneous processing of functional and non-functional action sequences. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 1(1), 18–30. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2010.550722
  • Nielbo, K. L., & Sørensen, J. (2013). Prediction error during functional and non-functional action sequences: A computational exploration of ritual and ritualized event processing. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 13(3-4), 347–365. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12342098
  • Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual research review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361–383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675
  • Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2014). Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 266–272. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031772
  • Polskaia, N., St-Amant, G., Fraser, S., & Lajoie, Y. (2020). A functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) examination of how self-initiated sequential movements become automatic. Experimental Brain Research, 238(3), 657–666. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-020-05742-w
  • Redgrave, P., Rodriguez, M., Smith, Y., Rodriguez-Oroz, M. C., Lehericy, S., Bergman, H., Agid, Y., DeLong, M. R., & Obeso, J. A. (2010). Goal-directed and habitual control in the basal ganglia: Implications for Parkinson's disease. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(11), 760–772. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2915
  • Rossano, M. J. (2012). The essential role of ritual in the transmission and reinforcement of social norms. Psychological Bulletin, 138(3), 529–549. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027038
  • Saling, L. L., & Phillips, J. G. (2007). Automatic behaviour: efficient not mindless. Brain Research Bulletin, 73(1-3), 1–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresbull.2007.02.009
  • Schjoedt, U., Sørensen, J., Nielbo, K. L., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Cognitive resource depletion in religious interactions. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 3(1), 39–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2012.736714
  • Servant, M., Cassey, P., Woodman, G. F., & Logan, G. D. (2018). Neural bases of automaticity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 44(3), 440–464. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000454
  • Slepian, M. L., Oikawa, M., & Smyth, J. M. (2014). Suppressing thoughts of evaluation while being evaluated. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 44(1), 31–39. https://doi.org/10.1111/jasp.12197
  • Sosis, R., & Handwerker, W. P. (2011). Psalms and coping with uncertainty: Religious Israeli women’s responses to the 2006 Lebanon War. American Anthropologist, 113(1), 40–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1433.2010.01305.x
  • Talmont-Kaminski, K. (2016). Malinowski’s magic and skinner’s superstition: Reconciling explanations of magical practices. In D. Xygalatas, & W. McCorkl (Eds.), Mental culture: Classical social theory and the cognitive science of religion (pp. 98–109). Routledge.
  • Teasdale, J. D., Dritschel, B. H., Taylor, M. J., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., Nimmo-Smith, I., & Baddeley, A. D. (1995). Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Memory & Cognition, 23(5), 551–559. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03197257
  • Teasdale, J. D., Proctor, L., Lloyd, C. A., & Baddeley, A. D. (1993). Working memory and stimulus-independent thought: Effects of memory load and presentation rate. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 5(4), 417–433. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541449308520128
  • Tonna, M., Marchesi, C., & Parmigiani, S. (2019). The biological origins of rituals: An interdisciplinary perspective. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 98(1), 95–106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.12.031
  • Trigueros, R., Aguilar-Parra, J. M., Álvarez, J. F., & Cangas, A. J. (2019). Adaptation and validation of the mind-wandering questionnaire (MWQ) in physical education classes and analysis of its role as mediator between teacher and anxiety. Sustainability, 11(18), 5081. https://doi.org/10.3390/su11185081
  • Verplanken, B., & Orbell, S. (2003). Reflections on past behavior: A self-report index of habit strength 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 33(6), 1313–1330. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2003.tb01951.x
  • Wang, D., Hagger, M. S., & Chatzisarantis, N. L. (2020). Ironic effects of thought suppression: A meta-analysis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(3), 778–793. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691619898795
  • Wegner, D. M. (1994). Ironic processes of mental control. Psychological Review, 101(1), 34–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.101.1.34
  • Wegner, D. M., & Erber, R. (1992). The hyperaccessibility of suppressed thoughts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(6), 903–912. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.63.6.903
  • Wegner, D. M., Erber, R., & Zanakos, S. (1993). Ironic processes in the mental control of mood and mood-related thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(6), 1093–1104. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.65.6.1093
  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53(1), 5–13. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.53.1.5
  • Weinstein, Y. (2018). Mind-wandering, how do I measure thee with probes? Let me count the ways. Behavior Research Methods, 50(2), 642–661. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-017-0891-9
  • Whitehouse, H. (1995). Inside the cult: religious innovations and transmission in Papua New Guinea. Clarendon Press.
  • Wilder, D. A. (1978a). Effect of Predictability on units of Perception and attribution. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4(2), 281–284. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616727800400222
  • Wilder, D. A. (1978b). Predictability of behaviors, goals, and unit of perception. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 4(4), 604–607. https://doi.org/10.1177/014616727800400422
  • Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor's reach. Cognition, 69(1), 1–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00058-4
  • Wu, T., Kansaku, K., & Hallett, M. (2004). How self-initiated memorized movements become automatic: A functional MRI study. Journal of Neurophysiology, 91(4), 1690–1698. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.01052.2003
  • Zor, R., Keren, H., Hermesh, H., Szechtman, H., Mort, J., & Eilam, D. (2009). Obsessive–compulsive disorder: A disorder of pessimal (non-functional) motor behavior. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 120(4), 288–298. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1600-0447.2009.01370.x

Reprints and Corporate Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

To request a reprint or corporate permissions for this article, please click on the relevant link below:

Academic Permissions

Please note: Selecting permissions does not provide access to the full text of the article, please see our help page How do I view content?

Obtain permissions instantly via Rightslink by clicking on the button below:

If you are unable to obtain permissions via Rightslink, please complete and submit this Permissions form. For more information, please visit our Permissions help page.